Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Relations of mother’s sense of coherence and childrearing style with child’s social skills in preschoolers

Abstract

Background

We examined the relationships between mothers’ sense of coherence (SOC) and their child’s social skills development among preschool children, and how this relationship is mediated by mother’s childrearing style.

Methods

Mothers of 1341 Japanese children, aged 4–5 years, completed a self-report questionnaire on their SOC and childrearing style. The children’s teachers evaluated their social skills using the social skills scale (SSS), which comprises three factors: cooperation, self-control, and assertion.

Results

Path analyses revealed that the mother’s childrearing mediated the positive relationship between mother’s SOC and the cooperation, self-control, and assertiveness aspects of children’s social skills. Additionally, there was a significant direct path from mother’s SOC to the self-control component of social skills.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that mother’s SOC may directly as well as indirectly influence children’s social skills development through the mediating effect of childrearing. The results offer preliminary evidence that focusing on support to improve mothers’ SOC may be an efficient and effective strategy for improving children’s social skills development.

Background

Sense of coherence (SOC), a concept developed by Antonovsky [1], refers to an individual’s personal ability to cope with stressors. Specifically, SOC has been defined as a ‘global orientation expressing the extent to which one has a pervasive, enduring though dynamic feeling of confidence that (i) the stimuli deriving from one’s internal and external environments in the course of living are structured, predictable, and explicable (comprehensibility); (ii) the resources are available to one to meet the demands posed by the stimuli (manageability); and (iii) these demands are challenges, worthy of investment and engagement (meaningfulness)’ [1, 2]. SOC is a theoretical concept that stems from salutogenesis, which focuses on what factors promote health and well-being (as opposed to factors that cause disease, which have been the focus of most models of health) [36]. SOC was conceived as a salutary factor (i.e. a health factor) according to the salutogenic model, which states that in order to obtain well-being, it is important for people to focus on their own resources and capacity for coping [7]. The salutogenic model is an important contribution to the theoretical underpinnings of health promotion, which is being advocated by the World Health Organization [8, 9].

SOC influences an individual’s resources for coping with stressful situations. Individuals are always exposed to stress—indeed, it is part of the human environment. Numerous studies on the relationship between SOC and stress have determined that people with a strong SOC tend to cope with stressful situations better, thereby leading to improved well-being and health status. People with high SOC also tend to perceive situations as manageable and meaningful, and view stressors as important challenges worth facing. They tend to be flexible and able to draw on appropriate resources to overcome a situation [10]. In contrast, people with poor SOC tend to be more vulnerable to stress and its negative health effects [5, 11, 12].

SOC appears to be closely associated with health, particularly mental health. SOC is, for instance, inversely related to psychological distress and psychiatric symptomatology (e.g. stress, depression, and anxiety); it appears to mitigate the negative impact of life stress [916]. Furthermore, SOC is positively related to psychological well-being [17] and quality of life [18]. On the other hand, poor SOC is related to life stress, psychological distress, and psychiatric symptomatology; it appears to enhance the negative impact of life stress [19]. Depending on levels of SOC, SOC can be either a positive or a negative factor that affects mental health functioning.

SOC also appears to be significantly associated with parenting stress (e.g. stress originating from the parenting role) [20]. Specifically, SOC appears to be positively related to parents’ self-esteem and inversely related to parental stress and depression [21, 22]. Parents are faced with multiple stressors throughout their child’s development, such as decisions on what constitutes effective parenting strategies, managing child behaviour, financial responsibilities, health concerns, and educational responsibilities [23]. Studies suggest that such stressors have a major impact on the child [23, 24]. Especially, maternal stress demonstrated notable negative impacts on children’s outcomes [24]. Maternal stress specifically appears to affect children’s social functioning and strongly predicts their adjustment, including development of internalizing (e.g. anxiety and depression) and externalizing disorders (e.g. inattention, defiance, impulsivity, and aggression) [2530].

Parental stress might affect children’s social development through its effect on parenting behaviours. Parenting stress has been found to predict dysfunctional parenting practices, including negative parenting styles [27] and poorer parenting behaviours [3133], which in turn are associated with problematic child behaviours [26, 34]. Parents with higher stress levels display less responsiveness in their parent–child interactions and more authoritarian parenting styles (e.g. overly strict and controlling), and show an increased risk of child maltreatment (e.g. harsh verbal and physical disciplining practices) [3437]. These negative parenting styles are associated with poor behavioural, socio-emotional, and cognitive outcomes among children, and can negatively influence coping skills among children whose parents have high levels of parenting stress [23]. Additionally, they can lead to more emotional, behavioural, cognitive, and physical problems throughout the child’s development [34].

Given the above theory, we propose that mother’s SOC may influence children’s social development indirectly through its influence on parenting practices. However, despite SOC’s important role in parenting stress and the fact that such stress has been demonstrated to negatively influence child development, there has been very little research on the mechanisms of the relationship between mother’s SOC and children’s development. Several reports have established relationships between parental SOC and children’s social development [21, 38], but the number is paltry in absolute terms. Particularly, there is little research focusing on the relationships between parental SOC and children’s development in preschool children.

Parental SOC not only may affect children’s social development through parenting attitudes and behaviours, but also may directly affect it through demonstration of better coping methods to children. In other words, children may benefit from perceiving the life orientation of a parent with high SOC, as such an orientation may be connected to flexible and successful coping. This may also lower the risk of negative outcomes for children, such as social maladjustment. However, as noted before, there is comparatively little research on the influence of parents’ SOC on child development among preschoolers, which makes the above mere speculation.

The development of social skills in early childhood is an important area of research in the field of child development, as such skills are essential for social competence [3941]. Social competence is defined as an individual’s ability to function in relation to other people, particularly with respect to getting along with others and forming close relationships [42]. It is also viewed as the ability to understand others in the context of social interactions and engage in smooth communication with others [43]. Social competence has been shown to be an important protective factor for children, as it is a buffer against stress and thereby helps to prevent serious emotional and behavioural problems later in life [44]. Deficits in social skills (e.g. cooperation, self-control, and assertion) in early childhood are relatively stable over time, and appear to relate to problems such as externalizing and internalizing disorders and poor academic performance, which are in turn precursors to more severe problems in the future [4549].

The development of social skills is determined by complex interactions between the individual, home and school environments, peer relationships, and sociocultural background [50]. Primarily, however, children develop their social and emotional competence through interactions with others. Indeed, such skills are likely heavily dependent in early childhood on the family context, including parental involvement [24, 51]. Although mother’s SOC and parenting style may affect children’s social competence development, few studies have comprehensively confirmed this relationship. Thus, it is important to examine the associations among mother’s SOC, childrearing style, and child social skills development in a comprehensive model.

Current study

We aimed to clarify the relationship between mother’s SOC and children’s social skills development among preschoolers, particularly whether this relationship is mediated by childrearing style. We hypothesized the following pathways: (1) an indirect pathway between mother’s SOC and child’s social skills development through mother’s childrearing style; and (2) a direct pathway between mother’s SOC and child’s social skills development after controlling for childrearing style.

Methods

Participants

In 2013, self-report questionnaires were administered to mothers of preschool children (n = 1845) aged 4–5 years in 21 nursery schools and 10 kindergartens in Kyoto, a highly urbanized metropolis in Japan. Of those 1845 mothers, 1362 completed the questionnaires.

In the present paper, to accurately clarify the associations between mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, child’s social skills (i.e. cooperation, self-control, and assertion), the following were excluded from the analysis: (1) children diagnosed with developmental problems (these children had already been diagnosed at medical institutions before this study started), and (2) children whose mothers did not return completed questionnaires. For inclusion in this study, mothers did not have to be the target child’s biological parent; however, they did need to reside with the child. Of the 1362 children’s mothers who completed questionnaires, we excluded 21 because the children had a diagnosed developmental disorder. Thus, 1341 met the inclusion criteria. The children’s data were analysed in this study.

Ethics statement

Informed consent was obtained from all mothers and teachers prior to the start of this research. They were informed of the purpose and procedures of the study, and were made aware that they were not obliged to participate. Ethical approval was obtained from Kyoto University’s ethics committee in Kyoto, Japan (E1701).

Measures

The demographic information collected included mother’s and child’s age and sex, family structure, and perceived family economy.

Predictor: mother’s sense of coherence

The short version of the sense of coherence scale (SOC-13) [1, 52] was used. This is a short form of the original 29-item scale (SOC-29) and has demonstrated reliability and validity [53]. The scale has been validated for the Japanese population [54, 55]. The scale comprises 13 items measuring the domains of comprehensibility (5 items, e.g. ‘Has it happened in the past that you were surprised by the behaviour of people whom you thought you knew well?’), manageability (4 items, e.g. ‘Has it happened that people whom you counted on disappointed you?’), and meaningfulness (4 items, e.g. ‘Do you have the feeling that you really don’t care about what is going on around you?’). Items are rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 to 7. The SOC score is obtained by summing the 13 items; SOC is regarded as a unitary construct, and higher scores indicate a stronger SOC. The measure has adequate internal consistency and construct validity [53, 55]. In this study, the internal consistency was .82. In addition, the quality of the factor analysis models was assessed using the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test and Bartlett ́s test for sphericity. The KMO test measures the degree of multicollinearity between the included items and varies between 0 and 1; the recommended minimum is .50 [56]. In this study, the KMO value was .86. This was an acceptable KMO value. Bartlett’s test is a measure of the probability that the initial correlation matrix is an identity matrix and should be <.05. In this study, Bartlett’s Test was significant, indicating that the matrix does not resemble an identity matrix, further supporting the existence of factors within the data. In addition, homoscedasticity was inspected through Levene’s test; the test was greater than .05. Levene’s test was not significant, indicating results that showed homoscedasticity. The total score was converted to a z-score for the analysis.

Mediator: mother’s childrearing style

The Index of Child Care Environment (ICCE) is a 13-item measure of childrearing style [57]. The scale comprises 13 items that measure the level of human stimulation, social stimulation, avoidance of restriction, and social support provided in a child’s environment (e.g. ‘How often do you play with your child?’ ‘How many times have you hit or kicked your child?’). The ICCE was originally created and developed in Japan. This scale is based on the home observation for measurement of the environment, which is used to evaluate the quality and quantity of stimulation and support available to children in their home environment [58]. Each item is assessed using a multiple-choice format, and the answer is given a binary score according to the manual (1 = good, 0 = not good or not sure); the overall score is calculated by summing all item scores. A higher score indicates better childrearing. The measure has adequate internal consistency and construct validity [57]. In this study, the internal consistency was .71. In addition, in this study, the KMO value was .79, Bartlett’s test was significant, and Levene’s test was not significant; these values indicated that assumptions of sphericity and homoscedasticity were met. The total score was converted to a z-score.

Criterion variable: child’s social skills

The social skills scale (SSS) is a 24-item measure of children’s social competence in terms of ‘cooperation’ (8 items, e.g. ‘Helps friends when asked’), ‘self-control’ (8 items, e.g. ‘Postpones gratification when requested’), and ‘assertiveness’ (8 items, e.g. ‘Expresses appropriate greetings to others’) [43, 59], which are all factors affecting social adaptation in later life [60]. In this study, children’s teachers were recruited to evaluate their social skills using this scale. The three subscales positively correlate with the scores of the child development scale [43, 59], which is based on the Social Skills Rating System [60]. Items are rated on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 to 2; the item scores are summed for each subscale to arrive at total scores for assertiveness, self-control, and cooperation. Higher scores indicate better social skills. The measure has adequate internal consistency and construct validity [43, 59]. In this study, internal consistency ranged from α = .83–.93. In addition, in this study, the KMO values were excellent in their range (cooperation; .93, self-control; .93, assertion; .90), the Bartlett’s Tests were significant, and the Levene’s tests were not significant; these values indicated that assumptions of sphericity and homoscedasticity were met. Each SSS total score was converted to a z-score.

Procedure

There were 260 nursery schools and 122 kindergartens in Kyoto city, Japan. We asked the facilities and conducted our survey at facilities where permission was obtained. To recruit families, self-reported questionnaires were distributed to all parents of preschool children (n = 1845) aged 4–5 years in the 21 nursery schools (12 private nursery schools and 9 public nursery schools) and 10 kindergartens (10 private kindergartens). The principals of each participating facility gave permission for us to meet with the parents. The participants received an information sheet and questionnaires with their child’s ID number. Participants provided written informed consent and agreed to participate. The parents completed the questionnaires at a single time point and returned these to participating facilities in sealed envelopes to prevent the teachers from seeing the questionnaires. Then, the child’s teacher checked the ID number of each received questionnaire, and the teachers evaluated the children’s social skills using the SSS.

Data analyses

Correlational analysis was performed to measure associations between mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, child’s social skills (i.e. cooperation, self-control, and assertion), and demographic characteristics (i.e. child’s sex, child’s age, mother’s age, presence of father, presence of siblings, attending kindergarten, and perceived family socioeconomic status). Path analyses were then conducted to estimate direct and indirect paths between mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, and child’s social skills. In the models, mother’s SOC was specified as a predictor of (a) mother’s childrearing style and (b) child’s social skills. Prior to estimating the full model (Fig. 1), a partial model that did not include mother’s childrearing style was estimated. All observed variables are enclosed in boxes and unobserved variables in ellipses. The unobserved variables are error terms. Error terms are associated with all endogenous variables and represent measurement error along with effects of variables not measured in the study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothesized model. This model includes the hypothesized pathways between mother’s sense of coherence, mother’s childrearing, and children’s social skills (cooperation, self-control, and assertion). All observed variables are in boxes, and error terms (e1–e5) are in ellipses

To assess fit, we used the comparative fit index (CFI) [61], incremental fit index (IFI) [62], and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) [63]. Good fit is reflected by CFI and IFI values above .90 [61, 62] and RMSEA values of .08 or less [64]. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 22.0 and AMOS version 23.0.

Results

Descriptive statistics

In this study, 1341 mothers and children were analysed. For children, there were 649 girls (48.4%); 680 (50.7%) were 4-year-olds, while 661 (49.3%) were 5-year-olds. The mothers’ ages ranged from 22 to 50 (M = 36.82, SD = 4.75). In terms of presence of father, 1221 (91.1%) children resided with father. In terms of presence of siblings, 1041 (77.6%) children had siblings. In terms of children’s attendance, 717 (53.5%) children attended kindergartens, while 624 (46.5%) children attended nursery schools. In terms of perceived family socioeconomic status, 134 mothers (10.0%) classified themselves as very poor, 164 (12.2%) as poor, 860 (64.1%) as fair, 156 (11.6%) as good, and 27 (2.0%) as very good.

Descriptive statistics for all study variables are presented in Table 1. Correlation analysis was used to determine relations between the main variables (i.e. mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, child’s social skills) and the demographic variables (i.e. child’s sex, child’s age, mother’s age, presence of father, presence of siblings, attending kindergarten, and perceived family socioeconomic status) to identify potential control variables in Table 2. All of the main variables were significantly correlated. Furthermore, significant correlations were also found between the demographic variables except mother’s age, and the dimensions of social skills (i.e. cooperation, self-control, and assertion). Specifically, child’s sex was significantly related to cooperation (r = .19, p < .001), self-control (r = .27, p < .001), and assertion (r = .11, p < .001), as was child’s age (cooperation, r = .25, p < .001; self-control, r = .22, p < .001; assertion, r = .14, p < .001), presence of father (cooperation, r = .09, p < .01; self-control, r = .09, p < .01; assertion, r = .06, p < .05), presence of siblings (cooperation, r = .09, p < .01; self-control, r = .05, p < .05; assertion, r = .05, p < .05), attending kindergarten (cooperation, r = .10, p < .001; self-control, r = .07, p < .05; assertion, r = .05, p < .05) and family socioeconomic status (cooperation, r = .06, p < .05; self-control, r = .07, p < .05; assertion, r = .10, p < .01). Therefore, these demographic variables were entered into the predictive models as control variables.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for the study variables (N = 1341)
Table 2 Correlations between mother’s sense of coherence, mother’s childrearing, child’s social skills, and demographic characteristics

Hypothesized paths

To test the mediating effect, we tested three models (Table 3). In Model 1, the standardized direct effect of mother’s SOC on child’s social skills, without controlling for mother’s childrearing style, was statistically significant (cooperation, β = .34, p < .05; self-control, β = .32, p < .01; assertion, β = .19, p < .05). In Model 2, the study indicated the effect of mother’s SOC on child’s social skills in the full mediation model was both directly and indirectly statistically significant. Therefore, mother’s childrearing style appears to be a mediator in the relationship between mother’s SOC and child’s social skills.

Table 3 Coefficients for path analyses

In Model 2, several statistically significant direct paths were found between the predictors and criterion variables (Fig. 2). First, mother’s SOC was found to be a significant predictor of mother’s childrearing style (β = .20, p < .001) and child’s self-control (β = .21, p < .01). Mother’s childrearing style was also found to be a significant predictor of child’s cooperation (β = .26, p < .01), self-control (β = .21, p < .05), and assertiveness (β = .20, p < .01). Therefore, mother’s SOC was found to indirectly relate to child’s social skills through mother’s childrearing style. Additionally, according to the fit indices, the full model fit the data well [χ2 (22) = 148.84, CFI = .94; IFI = .94; RMSEA = .06]. Figure 2 displays the final model and the standardized path coefficients.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Statistically significant paths. This model includes paths that were statistically significant in the hypothesized model. Path analyses controlled for child sex and age, presence of father, presence of siblings, attending kindergarten, and perceived family socioeconomic status. All observed variables are in boxes, and error terms (e1–e5) are in ellipses. Model fit statistics: χ2 (22) = 148.84, CFI = .94; IFI = .94; RMSEA = .06. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001

Discussion

We examined the correlations between mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, and child’s social skills among preschool children, and tested whether mother’s childrearing was a mediator in the relationship between the other two variables. Our hypothesized model was confirmed, in that mother’s SOC was a significant predictor of social skills through mother’s childrearing style. In addition, notably, mother’s SOC was a significant predictor of social skills directly, even after adjusting for mother’s parenting.

Indirect path between variables

Mother’s SOC was positively linked with cooperation, self-control, and assertiveness indirectly through mother’s childrearing style. This result is consistent with previous research findings, as related in the following sections.

Association between mother’s SOC and childrearing style

Mother’s SOC was positively linked with mother’s childrearing style, which has been indirectly supported in previous findings. As noted previously, individuals with a strong SOC were more likely to perceive their lives as less stressful [19], and SOC appears to be inversely related to parenting stress [21]. Parents with strong SOC are likely to select more active coping strategies and report more positive feelings toward positive parenting, such as emotional expressiveness, responsiveness, and support [65, 66]. Positive parenting has been shown to enhance empathy and children’s social functioning [6770]. In contrast, parents with poor SOC are likely to be vulnerable to parenting stress [21]. Parenting stress influences parenting behaviours that are not developmentally appropriate, inconsistent discipline (i.e. alternating between too lax and too harsh), and a lack of warmth and responsiveness in parent–child interactions [3133, 35, 71, 72]. Furthermore, parents experiencing high levels of stress are typically less responsive and affectionate with their children and more likely to use power-assertive discipline strategies and hold hostile parental attitudes, as compared with parents who can effectively cope with their stress [73]. Parenting stress is also presumed to interfere with parenting practices that help regulate children’s behaviour and emotions [74]. Therefore, parents with a stronger SOC who can effectively cope with their stress, are likely to have more positive parenting styles, whereas parents with a poorer SOC who are more vulnerable to stress, are more likely to have more negative parenting styles.

Furthermore, individuals with a stronger SOC use personal and emotional intelligence in proper ways to smoothly handle multiple demands and quickly adapt to their social environments [75]. Parents with higher SOC would make better choices, more adeptly manage their lives, and face fewer problems in life events, and have less time to solve them, than those with poorer SOC [76, 77]. Thus, higher SOC is likely to mean less time devoted to dealing with such problems and their consequences, and consequently, more time and availability to care for their children. On the other hand, worse SOC may mean less time and availability to care for children, or at least worse care. For the above reasons, mother’s SOC may be positively linked with mother’s childrearing style.

Association between mother’s childrearing style and child’s social skills

We also found an association between mother’s childrearing style and specific social skills—cooperation, self-control, and assertiveness—among children. This result is consistent with previous findings that parenting is an important contributor to children’s social development [70]. For example, greater parental warmth and sensitivity predicts greater emotional sensitivity, perspective taking skills (i.e. awareness and understanding of other people’s situations), and prosocial behaviours among children [7880]. Furthermore, parents who use consistent discipline such as firm rules and structure while encouraging development of mastery and independence appear to have more socially competent children [74]. Children whose parents are supportive, emotionally available, and teach their children effective emotion regulation strategies and coping skills are more likely to be socially competent and less prone to experience negative emotions/behaviours with peers [8183].

In contrast, the use of power-assertive discipline strategies and hostile parental emotions appear to be negatively associated with empathic and prosocial development [78], while stricter control predicted somewhat lower levels of positive social behaviour [79]. For instance, parents who are overly strict and controlling might place undue demands on children, which might cause children to develop negative affect (e.g. anger) and engage in more self-focused thoughts and actions [79]. Furthermore, parental control mixed with harsh verbal and physical disciplining practices can lead to aggressive and antisocial behaviours [79] or other problem behaviours among children [84].

Thus, a warm and supportive parenting style is viewed as an important resource associated with positive developmental outcomes, whereas overly controlling parenting is associated with negative outcomes. Therefore, mother’s SOC (that is, the ability to manage stress), through its effect on parenting style, can affect a child’s social development.

Direct path between mother’s SOC and child’s social skills

Interestingly, mother’s SOC was directly positively linked with the development of self-control. This result accords with the results of previous studies, indicating that parents’ personal ability is associated with a child’s social development [38, 78, 85]. A possible direct mechanism of the effect of mother’s SOC on children’s development is modelling. Social learning theory suggests that children’s social development is influenced by modelling of behaviours and attitudes of significant others in their lives [86, 87]. Thus, child social development is likely to be positively related to parents’ personal ability via the effects of modelling [88].

Consistent with the theory, the direct relationship between mother’s SOC and children’s social skills might be due to the effects of modelling. Several studies on self-regulation of emotions suggest that parents provide significant models by which children learn to express emotions and later learn to control emotional expressivity [89, 90]. SOC helps individuals to express and control emotions effectively, by facilitating various resources within the individuals to cope with life events. Parents are faced with multiple stressors and they may differ in their responses to these stressors—some parents with higher SOC are able to deal with challenges more effectively while others with lower SOC display emotional intensity or become more inappropriately reactive. For instance, children whose parents display a wide range of positive and negative emotions in appropriate social contexts, are more likely to be able to learn how to express emotions that are appropriate to display in particular situations [91]. As a result, children are more likely to be able to control their emotions effectively and efficiently. In contrast, children whose parents display higher levels of anger or personal distress, are less likely to be able to observe and learn appropriate ways to regulate and express their negative emotions [92, 93]. Therefore, parents with higher SOC might model positive and socially appropriate emotional responses to frustrating situations and provide adaptive emotional coaching. In contrast, parents with lower SOC might model undercontrolled, angry emotions in frustrating situations and respond in a punitive fashion to their children’s expression of negative emotions. Consequently, mothers’ SOC levels were directly positively linked with the development of self-control, by demonstrating and teaching coping methods to their children.

Limitations and future directions

The findings should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, this was a cross-sectional study. The cross-sectional design poses several restrictions that make it difficult to assume causality among the factors. Prior studies have found that children’s developmental characteristics influence mother’s SOC as well as the influence of mother’s personal ability on children’s developmental outcomes [9496]. Children’s mental health functioning and mother’s SOC are likely to influence each other. Thus, longitudinal research is needed in order to examine the effects of mother’s SOC on the later development of preschool children.

Second, the majority of the data (i.e. mother’s SOC, child-rearing style, and demographic information) were obtained only from mothers; therefore, there is a risk of a reporting bias. Specifically, single respondents’ views toward child-rearing style and demographic information such as perceived family socioeconomic status, may be skewed either more positively or negatively. Therefore, in future studies, more dissimilar informants’ reports, including those from fathers, in addition to mothers, will be needed to evaluate more exactly how family factors affect children’s mental health functioning.

Third, in the current study, we evaluated family socioeconomic status only using perceived (i.e. self-reported) family socioeconomic status. Numerous studies have consistently found childhood socioeconomic status has been associated with children’s developmental outcomes [9799]. The previous studies have consistently focused on three quantitative indicators to provide reasonably good coverage of the domains of interest: income, education, and occupational status. Therefore, in future studies, more proper scales, including family income, parent’s education, and occupational status, will be needed to evaluate more exactly how socioeconomic status affects child mental health functioning.

Fourth, there are likely to be several other factors that were not accounted for in our model. Although we found the hypothesized effects of mother’s SOC on child mental health functioning, we did not consider certain other mother’s social abilities in our model. According to social learning theory, parents’ personal abilities influence children’s social functioning by modelling of behaviours [86, 87]. Mothers’ social skills might affect their children’s social skills through both modelling and interaction with the children. In addition, although children’s social competence is influenced by their modelling of behaviours and attitudes of significant others in their lives, we did not consider social characteristics of fathers and teachers, such as their SOC, in our model. Children spend much time together not only with mothers but also their fathers and teachers; hence, their fathers’ and teachers’ personal abilities might significantly influence children’s developmental outcomes.

Furthermore, although we did not consider genetic factors in our model; it is important to realize children’s social competence may be influenced by genetic risks as well as their environmental factors. Considerable evidence supports the conclusion that children’s mental health functioning is moderately heritable [100, 101]. The extent to which children’s mental health functioning is affected by environmental factors depends on genetic characteristics [102, 103]. Consequently, there are likely to be other factors that need to be included in this model. Future studies should investigate this possibility further by including more factors related to children’s mental health functioning.

Finally, these findings may not be generalizable to all families, because the sample was drawn from a limited geographical area in an urban metropolis in Japan. The reproducibility of the current results should be confirmed using data from other regions in a variety of settings. In summary, future research on these topics would benefit from longitudinal designs and samples with greater demographic and clinical diversity.

Conclusions

This study examined the interrelations between mother’s SOC, mother’s childrearing style, and child’s social skills in early childhood. We found significant direct paths from mother’s SOC to mother’s childrearing style and child’s social skills. These findings advance our understanding of how mother’s SOC and parenting affect children’s development according to a family systems perspective. Lacking social skills in early childhood puts children at risk for social maladjustment [46, 47, 104]. Therefore, focusing on support and education to maintain and improve mothers’ SOC, especially mothers with lower SOC, may be an efficient and effective strategy for improving children’s social adjustment.

Abbreviations

SOC:

The Sense of Coherence Scale

ICCE:

The Index of Child Care Environment

SSS:

The Social Skills Scale

References

  1. Antonovsky A. Unraveling the mystery of health: how people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Lindström B, Eriksson M. Salutogenesis. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2005;59:440–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Antonovsky A. The structure and properties of the sense of coherence scale. Soc Sci Med. 1993;36:725–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Antonovsky A. The salutogenic model as a theory to guide health promotion. Health Promot Int. 1996;11:11–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Eriksson M, Lindström B. Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and the relation with health: a systematic review. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2006;60:376–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Griffiths CA, Ryan P, Foster JH. Thematic analysis of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence theory. Scand J Psychol. 2011;52:168–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Antonovsky A. Health, stress, and coping: new perspectives on mental and physical well-being. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers; 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Kickbusch I. Tribute to Aaron Antonovsky—‘What creates health’. Health Promot Int. 1996;11:5–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Eriksson M, Lindström B. A salutogenic interpretation of the Ottawa Charter. Health Promot Int. 2008;23:190–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Antonovsky A. Can attitudes contribute to health? Advances. 1992;8:33–49.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Lundberg O, Nyström Peck M. Sense of coherence, social structure and health: evidence from a population survey in Sweden. Eur J Publ Health. 1994;4:252–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Kivimäki M, Feldt T, Vahtera J, Nurmi J. Sense of coherence and health: evidence from two cross-lagged longitudinal samples. Soc Sci Med. 2000;50:583–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. McSherry WC, Holm JE. Sense of coherence: its effects on psychological and physiological processes prior to, during, and after a stressful situation. J Clin Psychol. 1994;50:476–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Olsson MB, Larsman P, Hwang PC. Relationships among risk, sense of coherence, and well-being in parents of children with and without intellectual disabilities. J Policy Pract Intellect Disabil. 2008;5:227–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Pallant J, Lae L. Sense of coherence, well-being, coping and personality factors: further evaluation of the sense of coherence scale. Pers Individ Differ. 2002;33:39–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Lundberg O. Childhood conditions, sense of coherence, social class and adult ill health: exploring their theoretical and empirical relations. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:821–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sagy S, Antonovsky A, Adler I. Explaining life satisfaction in later life: the sense of coherence model and activity theory. Behav Health Aging. 1990;1:11–25.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Eriksson M, Lindström B. Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and its relation with quality of life: a systematic review. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2007;61:938–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Flannery RB, Flannery GJ. Sense of coherence, life stress, and psychological distress: a longitudinal study of intervening variables. Soc Sci Med. 1990;25:173–8.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Mak WW, Ho AH, Law RW. Sense of coherence, parenting attitudes and stress among mothers of children with autism in Hong Kong. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2007;20:157–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cederblad M, Pruksachatkunakorn P, Boripunkul T, Intraprasert S, Höök B. Sense of coherence in a Thai sample. Transcult Psychiatry. 2003;40:585–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Olsson MB, Hwang CP. Sense of coherence in parents of children with different developmental disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2002;46:548–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Cappa KA, Begle AM, Conger JC, Dumas JE, Conger AJ. Bidirectional relationships between parenting stress and child coping competence: findings from the pace study. J Child Fam Stud. 2011;20:334–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Anthony LG, Anthony BJ, Glanville DN, Naiman DQ, Waanders C, Shatter S. The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behaviour and preschoolers’ social competence and behaviour problems in the classroom. Infant Child Dev. 2005;14:133–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Huang CY, Costeines J, Ayala C, Kaufman JS. Parenting stress, social support, and depression for ethnic minority adolescent mothers: impact on child development. J Child Fam Stud. 2014;23:255–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Morgan J, Robinson D, Aldridge J. Parenting stress and externalizing child behaviour. Child Fam Soc Work. 2002;7:219–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Deater-Deckard K, Scarr S, McCartney K, Eisenberg M. Paternal separation anxiety: relationships with parenting stress, child-rearing attitudes, and maternal anxieties. Psychol Sci. 1994;5:341–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. DuPaul GJ, McGoey KE, Eckert TL, VanBrakle J. Preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: impairments in behavioral, social, and school functioning. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:508–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Anastopoulos AD, Guevremont DC, Shelton TL, DuPaul GJ. Parenting stress among families of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1992;20:503–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Buodo G, Moscardino U, Scrimin S, Altoè G, Palomba D. Parenting stress and externalizing behavior symptoms in children: the impact of emotional reactivity. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2013;44:786–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Crawford A, Manassis K. Familial predictors of treatment outcome in childhood anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1182–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Creasey G, Reese M. Mothers’ and fathers’ perceptions of parenting hassles: associations with psychological symptoms, nonparenting hassles, and child behavior problems. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1996;17:393–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Rodriguez C, Green A. Parenting stress and anger expression as predictors of child abuse potential. Child Abuse Negl. 1997;21:367–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Deater-Deckard K. Parenting stress and child adjustment: some old hypotheses and new questions. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 1998;5:314–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Rodgers A. Multiple sources of stress and parenting behavior. Child Youth Serv Rev. 1998;20:525–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Belsky J, Woodworth S, Crnic K. Trouble in the second year: three questions about family interaction. Child Dev. 1996;67:556–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Chan Y. Parenting stress and social support of mothers who physically abuse their children in Hong Kong. Child Abuse Negl. 1994;18:261–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Togari T, Sato M, Otemori R, Yonekura Y, Yokoyama Y, Kimura M, et al. Sense of coherence in mothers and children, family relationships and participation in decision-making at home: an analysis based on Japanese parent-child pair data. Health Promot Int. 2012;27:148–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Campos J, Frankel C, Camras L. On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev. 2004;75:377–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Cavell TA, Kelley ML. The measure of adolescent social performance: development and initial validation. J Clin Child Psychol. 1992;21:107–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Gresham FM. Conceptual and definitional issues in the assessment of children’s social skills: implications for classifications and training. J Clin Child Psychol. 1986;15:3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Burt KB, Obradović J, Long JD, Masten AS. The interplay of social competence and psychopathology over 20 years: testing transactional and cascade models. Child Dev. 2008;79:359–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Anme T, Shinohara R, Sugisawa Y, Tanaka E, Watanabe T, Hoshino T. Validity and reliability of the social skill scale (SSS) as an index of social competence for preschool children. J Health Sci. 2013;3:5–11.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Garmezy N. Resilience and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes associated with poverty. Am Behav Sci. 1991;34:416–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Mash E, Barkley R, editors. Child psychopathology. New York: The Guilford Press; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:687–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Parker JG, Asher SR. Peer relations and later personal adjustment: are low-accepted children at risk? Psychol Bull. 1987;102:357–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Reiser M, Cumberland A, Shepard SA, et al. The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Dev. 2004;75:25–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Keane SP, Calkins SD. Predicting kindergarten peer social status from toddler and preschool problem behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2004;32:409–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Riggio RE. Assessment of basic social skills. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:649–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Early child care and children’s development prior to school entry: results from the NICHD study of early child care. Am Educ Res J. 2002;39:133–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Antonovsky, A. Kenko-no-nazo-wo-toku [Unraveling the mystery of health: how people manage stress and stay well] (Yamazaki Y, Yoshii K, Trans.). Tokyo: Yushindo Kobunsha; 1987.

  53. Eriksson M, Lindström B. Validity of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale: a systematic review. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2005;59:460–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Antonovsky A. Kenko-no-nazo-wo-toku [Unraveling the mystery of health: how people manage stress and stay well]. (Yamazaki Y, Yoshii K, Trans.). Tokyo: Yushindo Kobunsya; 2001.

  55. Yamazaki Y. Salutogenetic theory (a new theory of health) and the concept SOC (sense of coherence). Qual Nurs. 1999;5:81–8 (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  56. Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 2nd ed. London: SAGE; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Anme T, Tanaka E, Watanabe T, Tomisaki E, Mochizuki Y, Tokutake K. Validity and reliability of the Index of Child Care Environment (ICCE). Pub Health Front. 2013;2:141–5.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. HOME inventory administration manual. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences and University of Arkansas at Little Rock; 2003.

  59. Takahashi Y, Okada K, Hoshino T, Anme T. Developmental trajectories of social skills during early childhood and links to parenting practices in a Japanese sample. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0135357.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills rating system: manual. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:238–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Bollen KA. Overall fit in covariance structure models: two types of sample size effects. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:256–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: an interval estimation approach. Multivar Behav Res. 1990;25:173–80.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Soc Methods Res. 1992;21:230–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Kliewer W, Fearnow MD, Miller PA. Coping socialization in middle childhood: tests of maternal and paternal influences. Child Dev. 1996;67:2339–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Noojin AB, Wallander JL. Perceived problem-solving ability, stress, and coping in mothers of children with physical disabilities: potential cognitive influences on adjustment. Int J Behav Med. 1997;4:415–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Barber B, Stolz H, Olsen J, Collins A, Burchinal M. Parental support, psychological control, and behavioral control: assessing relevance across time, culture, and method: abstract. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2005;70:125–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Waylen A, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown S. Parenting and health in mid-childhood: a longitudinal study. Eur J Public Health. 2008;18:300–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Zhou Q, Eisenberg N, Losoya SH, Fabes RA, Reiser M, Guthrie IK, et al. The relations of parental warmth and positive expressiveness to children’s empathy-related responding and social functioning: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2002;73:893–915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Barnett MA, Gustafsson H, Deng M, Mills-Koonce WR, Cox M. Bidirectional associations among sensitive parenting, language development, and social competence. Infant Child Dev. 2012;21:374–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Karrass J, VanDeventer M, Braungart-Rieker J. Predicting shared parent-child book reading in infancy. J Fam Psychol. 2003;17:134–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Pinderhughes EE, Dodge KA, Zelli A, Bates JA, Pettit GS. Discipline responses: influences of parents’ socioeconomic status, ethnicity, beliefs about parenting, stress, and cognitive-emotional processes. J Fam Psychol. 2000;14:380–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. McLoyd V. The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: psychological distress, parenting and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 1990;61:311–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Masten A, Coatsworth J. The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: lessons from research on successful children. Am Psychol. 1998;53:205–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Mowlaie M, Mikaeili N, Aghababaei N, Ghaffari M, Pouresmali A. The relationships of sense of coherence and self-compassion to worry: the mediating role of personal intelligence. Curr Psychol. 2016. doi:10.1007/s12144-016-9451-1.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Bitsika V, Sharpley C. Development and testing of the effects of support groups on the well-being of parents of children with autism-II: specific stress management techniques. J Appl Health Behav. 2000;2:8–15.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Melnyk BM. Coping with unplanned childhood hospitalization: the mediating functions of parental beliefs. J Pediatr Psychol. 1995;20:299–312.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. DeRose LM, Shiyko M, Levey S, Helm J, Hastings PD. Early maternal depression and social skills in adolescence: a marginal structural modeling approach. Soc Dev. 2014;23:753–69.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Carlo G, Mestre MV, Samper P, Tur A, Armenta BE. The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parenting styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviors. Int J Behav Dev. 2011;35:116–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. De Wolff MS, van Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 1997;68:571–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Denham S, Mitchell-Copeland J, Strandberg K, Auerbach S, Blair K. Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: direct and indirect effects. Motiv Emot. 1997;21:65–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Denham S, Renwick S, Holt R. Working and playing together: prediction of preschool social-emotional competence from mother–child interaction. Child Dev. 1991;62:242–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Smith EP, Prinz RJ, Dumas JE, Laughlin J. Latent models of family processes in African American families: relationships to child competence, achievement, and problem behavior. J Marriage Fam. 2001;63:967–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Dodge K, Pettit G, Bates J. Socialization mediators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Dev. 1994;65:649–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Isley SL, O’Neil R, Clatfelter D, Parke RD. Parent and child expressed affect and children’s social competence: modeling direct and indirect pathways. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:547–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Kornhaber RC, Schroeder HE. Importance of model similarity on extinction of avoidance behavior in children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1975;43:601–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavior change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84:191–215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Whitbeck L. Modeling efficacy: the effect of perceived parental efficacy on the self-efficacy of early adolescents. J Early Adolesc. 1987;7:165–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Nyman M, Bernzweig J, Pinuelas A. The relations of emotionality and regulation to children’s anger-related reactions. Child Dev. 1994;65:109–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA. Mothers’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quat. 1994;40:138–56.

    Google Scholar 

  91. McCoy K, Cummings EM, Davies PT. Constructive and destructive marital conflict, emotional security and children’s prosocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:270–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Denham SA. Maternal emotional responsiveness and toddlers’ social-emotional competence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;34:715–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Grych JH, Fincham FD. Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: a cognitive-contextual framework. Psychol Bull. 1990;108:267–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Schnyder U, Büchi S, Sensky T, Klaghofer R. Antonovsky’s sense of coherence: trait or state? Psychother Psychosom. 2000;69:296–302.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Dabrowska A. Sense of coherence in parents of children with cerebral palsy. Psychiatr Polska. 2007;41:189–201.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Oelofsen N, Richardson P. Sense of coherence and parenting stress in mothers and fathers of preschool children with developmental disability. J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2006;31:1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Ensminger ME, Fothergill K. A decade of measuring SES: what it tells us and where to go from here. In: Bornstein MH, Bradley RH, editors. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003. p. 13–27.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Oakes JM, Rossi PH. The measurement of SES in health research: current practice and steps toward a new approach. Soc Sci Med. 2003;56:769–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002;128:490–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Bouchard TJ Jr. Genetic influence on human psychological traits: a survey. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2004;13:148–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17:271–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Pluess M, Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Dev Psychol. 2010;46:379–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Elliott SN, Busse RT. Social skills assessment and intervention with children and adolescents: guidelines for assessment and training procedures. School Psychol Int. 1991;12:63–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

RH designed and managed the study, performed the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. MS participated in the design of the study conception. TK administered and supervised overall conduct of the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge all of the children, parents, kindergarten teachers, and childcare professionals for their participation in this study.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rikuya Hosokawa.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hosokawa, R., Katsura, T. & Shizawa, M. Relations of mother’s sense of coherence and childrearing style with child’s social skills in preschoolers. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 11, 11 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-017-0147-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-017-0147-6

Keywords