Skip to main content

Links between mental health problems and future thinking from the perspective of adolescents with experience of depression and anxiety: a qualitative study

Abstract

Background

Depression and anxiety are common during adolescence and could have detrimental impacts on young people’s ability to make and implement plans for their future. However, to the best of our knowledge, no other study has adopted a qualitative approach in investigating these effects from the perspective of adolescents with lived experiences of depression and anxiety. We sought to understand how young people perceive and interpret the impact of mental health conditions on their thinking about the future.

Methods

We conducted semi-structured interviews with 19 adolescents aged 16–19 years in the UK (median age = 19, IQR = 1.5), who had a history of protracted periods of clinical or subclinical depression and/or anxiety. They were asked to reflect on how their ability to think about the future and the content of the future-related thinking was impacted during periods of poor mental health, compared with periods of feeling well. Interviews were transcribed verbatim and subjected to thematic content analysis.

Results

Five domains were identified. First, the impact of mood on future thinking capability focuses on reduced ability and motivation to engage in future thinking. Second, the impact of mood on images, thoughts, and feelings about the future includes the emotional valence of future-related thoughts, their vividness, structure, and the extent to which they intimated subjective feelings of control (i.e., agency). Third, social influences focuses on social factors that might ameliorate or exacerbate future thinking. Fourth, reflections on personal worries and expectations about the future captures personal interpretations of past worries and hopes and how future thinking affected mood. Finally, personal coping refers to how young people cope with the negative emotions that come with future thinking.

Conclusions

This study provided a nuanced and granular account of how depression and anxiety impacted young people’s future thinking based on their lived experiences. By highlighting the different ways that variations in future thinking were experienced as a function of depression and anxiety, our analysis highlighted new factors that should be considered in studies of adolescent mental health risk, which could inform the development of new therapeutic approaches.

Introduction

Adolescence is a transitional period of development involving both personal growth and life challenges, when the decisions young people make can have life-long consequences [1, 2]. It is characterised by a combination of unique physiological and psychological needs [3], for example, identity formation and personal development, as well as the marked increase in the demands and pressures of the social context [4].

Adolescence is also a period of a rapid increase in mental health problems, especially depression and anxiety [5, 6]. These conditions can produce states of mind that impact the ability to mentally re-experience personal events from the past and imagine events that could happen in the future, termed episodic cognition [7]. Research shows that the amount and quality of episodic cognition, especially future-related, is connected with adolescents’ ability to make realistic and effective plans [8,9,10,11,12]. Therefore, depression and anxiety could potentially reduce adolescents’ capability to clearly imagine the future or alter the content, structure, and valence of future-related thoughts, and, consequently, reduce their ability and motivation to make future plans [13].

The existing literature focuses on the content of future thinking and its links with depression in adults. Adult depression is associated with hopelessness about the future, both concurrently and longitudinally [14,15,16,17,18]. Similarly, an optimistic future outlook predicts a lower risk of subsequent depression, while negative and pessimistic expectations are related to higher depression symptoms [19,20,21,22]. States of mind related to depression are characterised by a reduced motivation to act in relation to the future and generalised negative expectations of future events [23,24,25,26]. Depressed individuals have reduced feelings of control over their future, and also report a lack of motivation and confidence to make decisions in pursuit of their desired future goals [27].

Anxiety disorders have detrimental impacts on future-related thinking as well. Research shows that anxiety disorders are associated with a cognitive bias to threat [28]. Therefore, anxious adults are more likely to interpret ambiguous information about the future as threatening [29,30,31,32,33]. In addition, research demonstrates a negativity bias where anxious adults judge that negative future events are more likely to happen to them than neutral or positive ones [34]. Moreover, anxious individuals are also more specific when speculating about negative events than about positive events [35]. As a result, anxious adults tend to delay and avoid making decisions about their future due to their negatively biased interpretations of and increased vigilance to unpredictable future events [36, 37].

Existing studies examining the relationships between future thinking and mental health in late adolescence generally investigate questions similar to those asked in the adult literature. Therefore, studies tend to focus on generic aspects of future thinking, such as hope, optimism, pessimism, and future expectations, and how they relate to depressive and anxiety symptoms. Similar to findings from adult studies, hopelessness, negative future expectations and a lack of agency are found to be associated with subsequent depression and anxiety symptoms in late adolescence [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48].

As adolescence is a unique and crucial developmental stage, the particular characteristics and needs of adolescents should be considered in research. Simply adopting the instruments developed for adults risks giving a limited picture of the links between mental health variations and future thinking in adolescence [49,50,51]. A study focusing on this developmental period is needed to explore and identify the unique patterns of future thinking and how it is linked with mental health problems during this time.

In this research, we took a necessary first step by undertaking a qualitative interview study with adolescents aged 16–19, who had experienced depression and/or anxiety, to explore the impact that poor mental health had on their future thinking at that time. The study specifically sought to answer three research questions: (1) How could pre-existing concepts of future thinking be enriched by perspectives of young people with a history of depression and/or anxiety? (2) How do young people describe future thinking during periods of poor and good mental health? (3) What do young people think about the link between future thinking and mood states? We did not set out to examine the themes that may be specific to either disorder due to their high comorbidity (i.e., depression and generalised anxiety; [52, 53]), but rather focused on exploring variations in the future thinking during the periods of poor mental health and in the well state.

Methods

Recruitment and participants

We adopted the qualitative approach, which was particularly well suited to the current study as it required adolescents to identify areas and issues in their own experience that may be missed if the study was based on other people’s accounts of adolescent experience or the adult-related literature more generally. We interviewed a community sample of adolescents, who had had a history of high depression and/or anxiety symptoms after age 12 but were managing well at the time of participating. This included either having a clinical diagnosis or subclinical elevated symptoms (self-reported) that had been present for three or more days in a typical week over at least three months. This approach enabled us to consider adolescents’ lived experiences of mental illnesses and to obtain their perspective at a time when they had developed clear insights to reflect upon their future thinking during a period of poor mental health.

The study was advertised via the university recruitment circular, social media and mental health charities. Adolescents who expressed an interest in the study were screened for moderate to severe depressive and anxiety symptoms using a questionnaire adapted from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS-PL; [54]). The participants were not known to any of the authors prior to their participation, and participants were aware of the aim of this research and that it forms part of a PhD project in a UK institution.

The ethical approval was obtained from the Health Faculties Research Ethics Sub-committee at King’s College London (HR/DP-20/21-21394). All eligible participants provided written informed consent.

The semi-structured topic guide

We identified and summarised six main concepts of future related thinking from the existing literature and structured the topic guide around these pre-specified concepts (Table 1). This was implemented flexibly with variations in the order and wording of the questions. Follow-up questions were added where appropriate to probe for more in-depth reflections and comments. The interviewer probed for a contrast of future thinking between periods of poor mental health and the well state throughout the session. The topic guide was pilot-tested with two adolescents (one male aged 19 years and one female aged 16 years), and questions were dropped or modified where necessary.

Table 1 The six concepts of future thinking covered in the topic guide

Procedure

All interviews were conducted remotely on Microsoft Teams and audio/video-recorded. Only the participant and the first author (PT, M.Sc., female) were present in the interviews. All participants were interviewed once in a single session. At the beginning of each interview, participants completed a demographic questionnaire that asked about their age, sex, geographic location, and education level. After the interview, participants were thanked and provided opportunities to ask questions. Each participant received a £10 shopping voucher as an appreciation for their time. Field notes were recorded after the interviews describing the researcher’s reflections on the conversation.

All interviews lasted between 25 and 43 min. The transcripts of the interviews were not returned to participants for comment or correction. Prior to the current study, the first author had previous experience of conducting interviews in another qualitative study and had then received training on qualitative research design, methods, data collection and data analysis.

Coding and analysis

We analysed the data using thematic analysis [55, 56] with a blended inductive/deductive approach. The initial generation of the codes was influenced by the six pre-defined concepts of future thinking identified from the literature review of the existing future thinking measures and the aspects they capture (i.e., Capability, Content, Frequency, Structure, Valence, and Salience/vividness). The codes were then refined and modified using the interview data. We worked deductively starting with a few codes based on the pre-defined concepts (e.g., lack of capability, a blurry image for the future, and negative repetitive thoughts about the future), but then inductively adding up new codes and developed the codes iteratively as we reviewed the data.

We used thematic analysis to facilitate a flexible yet detailed interpretation of the dataset and a blended approach to facilitate consideration of the data in relation to Frost’s model [57], alongside new interpretations that arose during the coding process. We drew on a critical realist framework in our interpretations of the data [58] as it is a middle ground between positivism (concepts that are derived from scientific observations and predictions) and constructionism (subject truths that are constructed via interactions). This position allows for an observation of ‘reality’, whilst acknowledging that unobservable thoughts, perceptions and knowledge may have driven the observed events and is thought to be an appropriate approach to understanding a specific psychological phenomenon [59].

We referred to the COnsolidated criteria for REporting Qualitative research checklist (COREQ; [60]) and Yardley’s [61] criteria of good qualitative research in conducting and reporting the current study. Our study demonstrated sensitivity to context by showing awareness and openness of the participants’ perspectives and the setting. The researchers remained mindful that our positionality may have influenced the participants’ answers and that the identified codes and themes originated from the participants’ narratives. The multidisciplinary research team had expertise in qualitative research and adolescent mental health (GP), child and adolescent psychiatry (ESB, KA) and affective disorders (PT, JPO). Through data analysis iterations, we undertook reflexive dialogues on potential sources of bias and our prior assumptions that may come from personal, research, and professional experiences with the research topic to improve the awareness of our subjective influences on the research process.

Crucially, we were able to show sensitivity to the data by carefully considering the meanings generated by the participants. Commitment and rigour was also demonstrated by the in-depth engagement through data collection and undertaking a detailed, in-depth analysis. Transparency was achieved by reporting all steps and including tables of codes and quotes for the reader to clearly see how the interpretation was derived from the data.

Interviews were transcribed verbatim and de-identified as soon as they were completed, and the first author started coding when the first five interviews were done. Further interviews were coded as they were conducted in an iterative process, and the authors discussed and agreed on when data saturation had been achieved (i.e., when further interviews would not add new valuable insights [62]). We regularly cross-checked between the co-authors to make sure the codes do not reflect a single person’s interpretation alone. The first author conducted the initial coding and generation of the themes. All co-authors participated in checking and refining the themes and domains. Data were managed and analysed using NVivo (Lumivero NVivo, Version 13, 2020, R1).

Results

Participant characteristics

Nineteen participants (16 females) aged 16–19 years (mean = 18.2, standard deviation; SD = 1.2) were recruited in the UK. Eight (42%) participants were White, five (26%) were Asian/Asian British, four (21%) were Black/Black British, and two (11%) were mixed race. Thirteen (68%) participants had a clinical diagnosis of depression and/or an anxiety disorder, and six (32%) had subclinical self-reported symptoms. The time between the first depression/anxiety episode and the interview ranged from 6 months to 6 years (mean = 3.1, SD = 1.9). The demographic characteristics of participants are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 The demographic characteristics of participants

Five domains were identified: domain one, the impact of mood on the future thinking capability; domain two, the impact of mood on images, thoughts, and feelings about the future; domain three, social influences; domain four, reflections on personal worries and expectations about the future; and domain five, personal coping. Each of them includes two to four themes (for a list of themes, see Table 3).

Table 3 Overview of the coding structure

Domain 1: The impact of mood on the future thinking capability

The concept of Capability refers to the quality of being able to think about the future. It includes the cognitive ability to engage in future thinking and the willingness and motivation to proactively think and plan for the future. Two themes were identified: varied ability and motivation and stuck in the past (see Table 4).

Table 4 Themes and quotes of ‘The impact of mood on the future thinking capability’ during poor mental health and well state

Varied ability and motivation

Participants frequently talked about a lack of physical and mental capacity to think about the future when they were depressed and/or anxious. Some attributed it to being focused on and overwhelmed by what was going on at the time, leaving no ‘space’ to think about the future at all.

“I was feeling like I couldn't go on any longer, cause it's feeling too low, too down. I was struggling so much that I couldn't think about the future. I knew the future was there, but I couldn't get out of the bubble.” [01; male, 18, clinical depression and anxiety]

Others avoided thinking about the future, as they feared it would further worsen their mental health.

“I would avoid thinking about it, because it would, one, make me more stressed, and two, make me more sad.” [06; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

Some participants also described the time of poor mental health as ‘passive living’, in ‘autopilot mode’ and ‘zombie working’, where they followed routines and worked on what had to be done, without considering the consequences of their actions in the future.

“I physically and emotionally couldn't imagine anything beyond. … It was more of a day-by-day thing, so it wasn't like five years into the future, not even a year, just literally taking each day at a time and trying to hold onto something each day.” [16; female, 19, clinical depression]

Stuck in the past

During the poor mental health state, adolescents reported dwelling upon the past more than the future. They tended to think more about the events that had happened, especially those with negative consequences, such as their past mistakes. The future was often less considered or not thought about at all.

“Whenever I thought about the future back then, it was never good. … What I thought would be only getting worse, never getting better. I would dwell upon the past; as for the future back then, I didn't even have an image.” [04; female, 16, clinical depression and anxiety]

During the well state, adolescents were more future-oriented and thought more about the future than the past. A few participants generally became more reflective and introspective and thought more about both the past and the future.

“I’ve learned that, forget about the past and move forward. I don't pay attention to what has happened because that's gone. You need to be thinking about tomorrow.” [01; male, 18, clinical depression and anxiety]

“I try to focus more on the future, but I also remember the past. The past has made me the person that I am.” [10; female, 19, clinical PTSD]

Domain 2: The impact of mood on images, thoughts, and feelings about the future

This domain captures the images, thoughts, reflections, and feelings adolescents had about the future and includes four themes: valence, vividness, agency and structure (see Table 5).

Table 5 Themes and quotes of ‘The impact of mood on images, thoughts, and feelings about the future’ during poor mental health and well state

Valence

During poor mental health states, adolescents’ future thinking was predominantly negative. Participants reflected that when they had been depressed and anxious, they had been irrationally negative about the future, afraid of academic failure, failing their own and others’ expectations and never feeling good enough.

“Mainly negative, I don't remember thinking anything positive.” [05; female, 19, clinical depression and anxiety]

During the well state, adolescents were generally hopeful for the future. They believed their expectations and hopes for the future would come true.

“I used to think that the future will also be bad. So like goals, targets, I never used to have those, just living for the moment. But right now, I've set up goals and targets, and I'm actually hopeful for the future and how it will be.” [02; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

Vividness

During the poor mental health states, participants described a disconnection with their positive future selves. Some were unable to visualise the future at all. Others described it in terms of ‘black, blank and blurry’ images. In contrast, they had vivid images of a negative future self and their fears and worries about the future were described in rich detail, which even felt like reality.

“Cause I was so negative being in depression, it just became like there was no light at the end of the tunnel. It was so blurred in the whole process that I couldn't think what am I moving towards?” [01; male, 18, clinical depression and anxiety]

“I could see myself just not having done well, coming back home and having to answer questions, just staying in my room and crying, because I couldn’t do it. It was more vivid than other images of the future.” [08; female, 19, clinical depression]

During the well state, adolescents saw their future in more detail and specificity and described more connections to a positive future self. They were able to generate clear images of a future self and speculate their future targets and plans to achieve them.

“I've got a lot of things that I look forward to. I'm always thinking one step ahead, about third year, and what speciality, what skills.” [05; female, 19, clinical depression and anxiety]

Agency

During poor mental health states, some participants described a lack of agency, where they did not feel in control of their future or their negative feelings. Some described it as being externally controlled, for instance, ‘a ghost or spiritual curse’, ‘a religious punishment’, and ‘a monster in my head’.

“Interviewer: Do you think you have control over the future?

Participant: No, not at all. … I felt like there was a huge lack of control, and I felt that in terms of every aspect. … That lack of control was the most frustrating part because it's dehumanising in a way. Every aspect of life, like appetite, sleeping, friends, walking, was affected.” [19; female, 19, clinical depression and anxiety]

In contrast, in the well state, adolescents felt more control of their future and automatic negative thoughts. They became more flexible with planning for the future and accepted that some of it was beyond their control. They were also able to see a longer-term and larger-scale future instead of overthinking the small things right in front of them. They used evidence and scientific thinking to combat worries and were able to distinguish perception from reality.

“I can't think of anything else that would play a bigger part than my own choices and my own vision.” [08; female, 19, clinical depression]

Structure

When thinking about the fears and worries for the future during the poor mental health state, participants described ruminating on and being with their thoughts in negative cycles. They worried that a single negative event would lead to a series of subsequent events or something even worse (serial thinking). They also felt negativity would be enduring while positivity was only temporary by constantly playing down their achievements and successes.

“I felt so out of control with my emotions that I felt like it was going to be like that forever. I would think, I've been like this for a month and a half, it's never gonna end, I'm gonna continue like this for the rest of my life.” [06; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

When well, some participants were able to shift their focus from negative fixated thoughts about minor details. This allowed them to let go of the negativities and regrets and free themselves from the vicious cycle of being caught up in rumination. Some described this as ‘feeling more focused again on what’s important’.

“I used to be so focused on overthinking the thing that was gonna come up. Now I can look at more things, at the bigger picture, so my whole life perspective, sequencing and timeline.” [05; female, 19, clinical depression and anxiety]

Domain 3: Social influences

The Social influences domain refers to the direct and indirect influences from young people’s family and peers on their future thinking. The social context has both negative and positive influences on participants’ future thinking. This domain includes two themes: social pressure and support from friends and peers, and pressure from parents and financial hardship (Table 6).

Table 6 Themes and quotes of ‘Social influences’

Social pressure and support from friends and peers

Talking about the future with friends and peers could bring fear and anxiety. Social pressures could exacerbate the anxiety about one’s future.

“There are those people who believe in you, and they believe you can really do it, but as always, we have people who’re so negative and they discourage you in everything. … Why do you think you are special? People failed before, why would you succeed?” [11; male, 18, clinical depression and PTSD]

However, the influences from friends and peers were described positively as well. Engaging other people in thinking about the future provided encouragement and inspiration and brought hope and excitement. Participants talked about having seen people close to them recover from depression and achieve their hoped-for future through hard work, which helped them feel more optimistic. Social support throughout depressive/anxiety periods also brought hope about the future.

“It’s a good inspiration, when I saw that person got out of depression, worked hard and succeeded, it motivated me to concentrate in my dreams and goals, and just stop getting sad most of the time. So I thought even my life can be that way. I had a more optimistic way of thinking.” [02; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

Pressure from parents and financial hardship

Family and parents made future thinking stressful for adolescents when the future they wanted for themselves deviated from the one their parents wanted them to have. This also made adolescents feel they lacked control over their future.

“My education wise, at that time, they [my parents] were making all my decisions for me. That’s why I felt like I had zero control of my future, and that whole thought process would spiral in my head all the time. I wouldn’t have considered [the future], because everything was done, all the decisions were made.” [04; female, 16, clinical depression and anxiety]

Growing up in a socioeconomically disadvantaged household also created more pressure to think about the future, as these adolescents had to plan how to create a more positive future for themselves.

“I think what worries me is money, if I’m not able to get a good job, to pay off my bills, to live the expectations of life I’m looking forward to, because I haven’t had that in life, I come from a deprived neighbourhood. I don’t wanna be on benefits. I wanna do something in life, I hope that it is a positive future ahead for myself.” [01; male, 18, clinical depression and anxiety]

Domain 4: Reflections on personal worries and expectations about the future

This domain captures participants’ reflections and interpretations about their past worries and hopes, and how different styles of future thinking affected their mood. It includes two themes: the impact of Future thinking on mood; and optimism, pessimism, and future expectations (Table 7).

Table 7 Themes and quotes of ‘Reflections on personal worries and expectations about the future’

The impact of future thinking on mood

Although the relationship between future thinking and mood can be bi-directional, participants said that there was a stronger impact of future thinking on mood than the other way around. However, the relationship was not straightforward. Future thinking could cause negativity and anxiety when they were in a depressed/anxious state, and even thinking positively about the future did not improve their mood. However, the participant who engaged in future thinking as a strategy to remain hopeful recalled it having a positive impact [19]. In the well state, there was generally less influence of future thinking on adolescents’ mood, as one participant said:

“I can think a lot further into the future without getting upset, and I guess I let myself have fun imagining things.” [18; female, 16, clinical mixed depression and anxiety episodes].

Optimism, pessimism, and future expectations

When reflecting on periods of depression and anxiety, participants talked about how surprised they were when their ‘unrealistic’ positive expectations came true, and how worries they firmly believed would come true did not. Participants looked back at how they had underestimated their possible future achievements and overestimated the chance of failure:

“Initially I thought I’d do really bad in my GCSEs, and that didn’t really come true. I thought I’d probably do quite bad in my A-levels, and I did better than I thought I would. And I thought I’d get to university, but I’ve done better than I thought I would.” [05; female, 19, clinical depression and anxiety].

They also reflected on over-worrying and overthinking in general and how they subsequently learnt from these reflections to combat current worries and fears. They became more conscious and aware that they may have exaggerated their chance of failure. As a result, they were able to identify over-worrying more sensitively and challenge those thoughts with past experiences:

“I’d tell myself that, looking back, I had nothing to worry about, it was all self-inflicted.” [04; female, 16, clinical depression and anxiety]

“I think that right now, I’m doing a lot better, and I’m seeing it like a learning experience because now I look back at it, I can analyse myself the way I felt, in order for me to avoid going back to that hole in the future.” [06; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

Domain 5: Personal coping

Personal coping refers to how young people cope with the negative emotions that come with future thinking. It includes two themes: engaging in future thinking as a strategy to combat depression; and scientific thinking, to challenge perception with evidence (Table 8).

Table 8 Themes and quotes of ‘Personal coping’

Engaging in future thinking as a strategy to combat depression

When comparing future thinking during the poor mental health and well states, participants talked about being able to focus more on the future and less on worries and fears during well periods. They could plan the future more flexibly and look at the bigger picture instead of fixating on recurring negative thoughts, being rigid about set plans and rejecting the alternatives. They also acknowledged that some things were out of their control and allowed for more freedom visions for the future.

“I realise how things can change in an instant, and it does impact how I think about the future in that, I try not to be as rigid with ‘this must happen, if I don’t get this job, I’m not going to be happy’. It also makes me realise that things happen and we don’t have that much control over them.” [07; female, 19, clinical anxiety]

Alongside diverting the focus from the past to the future, there was also more focus on the present, enjoying the moment, taking things slowly, emphasising the process more than the outcome, and a generally more flexible approach to planning what they wanted to achieve.

“It has allowed me to take things slow and not want to rush to get to a specific place. … Before, I would get more stressed if I didn’t get specific things done on time. Now I’m a lot calmer about it.” [06; female, 19, subclinical depression and anxiety]

Among the 19 participants interviewed, one participant [09; male, age 16, subclinical depression and anxiety symptoms] stood out. Unlike the other adolescents who lost the motivation and capability to think about their personal future when depressed, he actively engaged in future thinking as a strategy to deal with his depressive symptoms and had vivid imagery of his future self throughout poor mental health periods.

“There's a song, by BTS called Pied Piper. … So during my really depressing period, I would just listen to that song, and it kept me going. Even if I felt bad now, I knew it would end so that I could get into that future state. … So me thinking about bad stuff from the past or from now, I thought about the future to counteract it, to give me hope in that times.”

Whilst other participants described a disconnection with a positive future self and an inability to visualise images of the future self, he had worked to create a detailed and positive future outlook during poor mental health periods. He intentionally engaged in future thinking, especially the pictures of a positive hoped-for future self, to protect himself against low mood and improve his motivation and confidence to achieve the ideal future self.

“The detail of me being a mathematician in the future is extremely detailed. It’s me in front of a blackboard, with my hot chocolate and marshmallows, drinking, doing the problem on the board, just thinking really hard.”

Scientific thinking to challenge perception with evidence

Participants also actively used strategies to manage and combat automatic negative thoughts by facing and dealing with them rather than avoiding them. Participants talked about engaging in scientific thinking, by challenging their perception with evidence rather than falling into the ruminative cycle.

“I’m much better at analysing these thoughts and thinking, do they make sense? Are they logical or are they just something I don’t really need to worry about and I need to distract myself from?” [18; female, 16, clinical mixed depression and anxiety episodes]

It should be noted that adolescents in this study had either depression and/or anxiety symptoms, or both, in the past. Although the study did not set out to distinguish between the two, one participant explicitly commented on the distinct impact that depression and anxiety had on her [19; female, 19, clinical depression and generalised anxiety disorder]. She described not thinking about the future at all during depression, while anxiety being the opposite, overthinking and overanalysing the future:

“So for depression, it’s avoidance, denial to see the future; and anxiety, it’s overanalysing, overthinking and also irrational thoughts. Depression is very different in the way that the future is not even a consideration; anxiety is the polar opposite, it’s on your mind, it’s intrusive, it’s over control, like overpowering.”

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to explore the views of adolescents who had experienced depression and anxiety about how poor mental health impacted their future thinking and vice versa. We adopted a qualitative approach and identified key domains and themes in which future-related thinking was either influenced by or contributed to depressive and anxious symptoms.

First, we identified a lack of ability (not being able) and motivation (not wanting) to think about the future during periods of poor mental health, as well as a stronger orientation towards the past over the future. Some participants suggested that together, the cognitive inability and emotional avoidance of thinking about the future may have prevented depression and anxiety from getting worse (“because it would, one, make me more stressed, and two, make me more sad.” [06]), as well as being a consequence of the low mood (“depression really hit me hard, I was mostly just zombie-working.” [11]). Previous studies have also reported the loss of motivation and struggles with maintaining and actively engaging with long-term goals. They have demonstrated that depressed adolescents have blunted neural responses in their anticipation and efforts to gain reward and avoid punishers [63,64,65]. Few existing studies specifically investigated the lack of ability and motivation to engage in future thinking and mostly quantified the ability of future thinking in relation to the time needed to generate future events or the clarity of elaborated future events [51, 66,67,68,69,70]. Moreover, in this study, we found that young people were predominantly past-oriented during a depressed and anxious state, which is in line with existing research that depression is associated with an increased orientation towards the past and an avoidance of the future [71,72,73].

Second, young people’s perspectives of what their future thinking was like during depressive and anxious episodes presented a broad picture of its content, including the valence, vividness, structure, and young people’s perceived sense of control. The thoughts related to the future were predominantly negative, which is consistent with previous research that depression and anxiety are related to an increased generation of negative future events [34, 74]. It is worth noting that apart from increased negative emotions towards the future, some studies have also reported an absence of positive emotions or any emotions at all, which leads to feeling empty, dull, and blank [75,76,77]. This was captured in our findings of vividness, where young people described a disconnection with a positive future self and consequently, having a blank image of the future and losing track of their goals. However, as one participant [19] described, future thinking in anxiety is characterised by heightened worries and repetitive negative anticipation of future events and is completely different compared with depression [35]. The future may be perceived as closer than the past in anxiety, whereas the opposite may be the case in depression, although only one participant in our study elaborated on this point [78]. Furthermore, poor mental health state was related to a shift of focus from the bigger picture of the future to a fixation on small details (captured by structure), possibly reflecting a belief that the negativity will be constant and they will be ‘stuck’ in this state forever, which aligns with the loss of a wider perspective reported in Watson et al. [65].

We found mixed positive and negative influences of the social context on young people’s future thinking, where social support and pressure from significant others, including friends, peers, and parents, can both encourage and hinder their future thinking. It is worth noting that when talking about the support and encouragement from people around them, more young people referred to friends and peers than family members. In line with the study of Sica et al. [79], peer support possibly provides a beneficial effect on strengthening engagement with one’s future in early adulthood. However, for young people who are already indifferent and avoidant of future thinking, influences from peers may deepen their avoidance of the future and consequently hinder future planning. In our study, participants coming from a lower socioeconomic background reported having more pressure to plan for their own future and more worries about whether they would be able to live a life of their expected standard. This was consistent with findings from Fakkel et al. [80] that disadvantaged socioeconomic status is related to lower parental support and fewer positive future orientations.

On a more positive note, young people in our study reported having developed techniques and skills to adapt their future thinking and challenge negative repetitive thoughts from their experiences of past episodes of depression and anxiety. On reflection, they reported that their past worries and fears built up the strength and resilience to cope with new challenges of the future, allowing for increased flexibility with their future planning. This is particularly important given that existing research provides evidence for the protective effect of resilience, flexibility, and positive affect on a lessening of self-reported depressive symptoms and a stable remission from clinical depression in adults [81, 82]. Interestingly, participants reported a stronger influence from future thinking on mood than the other way around, while also acknowledging the impairing impacts depression and anxiety had on their future thinking. The dynamic pathway between future thinking and mental health needs to be further examined regarding the directionality and temporality.

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first qualitative study to explore adolescents’ views on their future thinking abilities and patterns and how these are related to depression and anxiety. Compared with previous studies which examined separate future-related thinking aspects, we provided a more comprehensive perspective on the role future thinking plays in adolescent mental health. We explored aspects of future thinking that may originate from or exacerbate poor mental health from the accounts of adolescents with lived experiences of clinical or sub-clinical depression and anxiety. Furthermore, our study allowed the freedom to divert away from the adult models so that we could adopt a more developmentally appropriate approach.

One strength of the study was that we recruited participants of different ethnicities, varied socioeconomic backgrounds, education levels and geographic locations in the UK. However, there might still be a selection bias as participants have to be currently managing well to be deemed eligible. We were therefore unable to explore future thinking among those who were experiencing depression or anxiety at the time of participation. Our eligibility criteria were based on the consideration that adolescents who are unwell might not have the capability or motivation to reflect on their future thinking. Also, a few sensitive questions in the topic guide might elicit concerns for their mental health and safety.

Including both clinical and subclinical symptoms allowed us to investigate lived experiences of depression and anxiety on a continuum rather than a dichotomy, which provides a good representation of experiences in real life. However, we should also acknowledge the challenges of relying on self-reports here. For example, the participant [09] who reported still having a positive future outlook and used it as a coping strategy to combat depression may have been in a state of poor mental health, rather than a state of mental illness. Therefore, whether a self-reported depressive episode could be interpreted as being mentally ill should be taken into account when interpreting the study’s findings. However, about two-thirds of participants reported having clinically diagnosed depression and/or anxiety and their reflections did not appear qualitatively different from those without a diagnosis. Finally, although the findings are based on retrospective accounts, we included adolescents who experienced depression and anxiety after age 12, their experiences therefore were not distant temporally.

We did not seek to distinguish between future thinking patterns that were unique to depression and anxiety, as the comorbidity rates for these disorders are high in adolescence (e.g., between 15.9 and 61.9%; [52, 53]). Depression and anxiety symptoms are also highly correlated, making it challenging to identify young people with only depression and not anxiety, and vice versa. According to some participants, it would be hard for them to distinguish between the influences of depression and those of anxiety on their future thinking.

Despite the important insights provided by the qualitative approach in terms of understanding personal accounts and interpretations, it has limitations too. The findings are based on the individuals’ subjective experiences and are context-specific. This means that the generalisability of the findings to a wider population is limited. However, this research constitutes a preparatory stage of a longitudinal project on adolescent mental health and future thinking. The coding and analysis of the interviews informed the development of a new quantitative measure specifically designed for adolescent mental health and future thinking. Participants were shown the candidate items for the new measure and provided feedback in a separate session (for a detailed explanation, see Tang et al. [83]). By identifying aspects of future thinking that could present as either potential risks for or consequences of poor mental health, findings from the present study inform the design of a longitudinal phase of the project.

Implications and next steps

From a clinical and practical perspective, this study demonstrated the profound impact that poor mental health has on adolescent decision-making and future planning, which was reflected in the narratives of participants. The findings emphasised the importance of supporting adolescents at the crucial stage of personal growth and life challenges. Given the relationship between poor mental health and reduced capability to engage in future thinking, the loss of intrinsic motivation and an overly pessimistic expectation may hinder young people from living up to their full potential. Therefore, young people who suffer from low mood and anxiety may need additional support to set up realistic, concrete and achievable future plans. Moreover, understanding the directionality between changes in future thinking patterns and adolescent mental health problems could provide important insight into identifying at-risk individuals and incorporating support with future thinking aspects in therapeutic approaches, such as promoting positive mental imagery and specificity in episodic future thinking, which were proven effective in treating clinical depression [84,85,86,87,88].

This study also extended the existing academic knowledge by providing a comprehensive and developmentally sensitive insight into a conceptual structure of future thinking in adolescent mental health problems. We identified future thinking patterns that are associated with depression and anxiety disorders in late adolescence, based on young people’s lived experiences. However, the causal pathways between future thinking and depression are yet to be determined. Although this was an exploratory qualitative study, it highlighted the factors to be measured in future quantitative studies, which could statistically test hypotheses about the direction and temporality of changes in future thinking patterns and mental health symptom severity.

Conclusions

We explored the perspectives of adolescents with lived experiences of depression and anxiety disorders regarding how their future thinking was affected by poor mental health, compared with when they manage well. Depression and anxiety affected the ability and motivation to engage in future thinking and the valence, vividness, structure, and agency concerning future-related thoughts. Young people’s reflections on their past worries, fears, and expectations revealed how they learnt through the process of better coping with poor mental health and their personal interpretations of the interrelated dynamics between mental health and future thinking. Findings from this study provided evidence that young people who had experienced poor mental health felt that it had profoundly impairing effects on their future thinking across multiple aspects. It also highlighted the importance of studying the temporal relationships between episodes of mental health problems and future-related thinking patterns to clarify potential causalities.

Availability of data and materials

The first author had full access to data used in the study. The data of this study with identifiable information removed are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.

References

  1. Reyna VF, Farley F. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2006;7(1):1–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52:83–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. World Health Organization. Adolescent pregnancy: adolescence is a time of opportunity during which a range of actions can be taken to set the stage for healthy adulthood: factsheet (No. WHO/RHR/14.08). 2014.

  4. Steinberg L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(2):69–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kessler RC, Amminger GP, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Lee S, Üstün TB. Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(4):359. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0B013E32816EBC8C.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK. Depression in adolescence. Lancet. 2012;379(9820):1056–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60871-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Cheke L. Episodic cognition: what is it, where is it, and when does it develop? 2012.

  8. Atance CM, O’Neill DK. Episodic future thinking. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5(12):533–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bromberg U, Lobatcheva M, Peters J. Episodic future thinking reduces temporal discounting in healthy adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11):e0188079. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0188079.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Bromberg U, Wiehler A, Peters J. Episodic future thinking is related to impulsive decision making in healthy adolescents. Child Dev. 2015;86(5):1458–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/CDEV.12390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Eskritt M, Doucette J, Robitaille L. Does future-oriented thinking predict adolescent decision making? J Genet Psychol. 2014;175(2):163–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2013.875886.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. McCue R, McCormack T, McElnay J, Alto A, Feeney A. The future and me: Imagining the future and the future self in adolescent decision making. Cogn Dev. 2019;50:142–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COGDEV.2019.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):837–44. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.60.8.837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Collazzoni A, Stratta P, Pacitti F, Rossi A, Santarelli V, Bustini M, Talevi D, Socci V, Rossi R. Resilience as a mediator between interpersonal risk factors and hopelessness in depression. Front Psychiatry. 2020. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Cuijpers P, De Beurs DP, Van Spijker BAJ, Berking M, Andersson G, Kerkhof AJFM. The effects of psychotherapy for adult depression on suicidality and hopelessness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2013;144(3):183–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Do DP, Beam Dowd J, Ranjit N, House JS, Kaplan GA. Hopelessness, depression, and early markers of endothelial dysfunction in U.S. adults. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(7):613–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181e2cca5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. O’Connor RC, O’Connor DB, O’Connor SM, Smallwood J, Miles J. Hopelessness, stress, and perfectionism: the moderating effects of future thinking. Cogn Emot. 2004;18(8):1099–120. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Rice KG, Leever BA, Christopher J, Porter JD. Perfectionism, stress, and social (dis)connection: a short-term study of hopelessness, depression, and academic adjustment among honors students. J Couns Psychol. 2006;53(4):524–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Chang EC. Optimism-pessimism and stress appraisal: testing a cognitive interactive model of psychological adjustment in adults. Cogn Ther Res. 2002;26(5):675–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Karhu J, Hintsanen M, Ek E, Koskela J, Veijola J. Dispositional optimism and pessimism in association with cognitive abilities in early and middle adulthood. Pers Individ Differ. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Prince DM, Epstein M, Nurius PS, Gorman-Smith D, Henry DB. Reciprocal effects of positive future expectations, threats to safety, and risk behavior across adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2019;48(1):54–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1197835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Robinson-Whelen S, Kim C, Maccallum RC, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? J Personal. 1997;73(6):1345.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Bradley B, Mathews A. Negative self-schemata in clinical depression. Br J Clin Psychol. 1983;22(3):173–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.2044-8260.1983.TB00598.X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hallford DJ, Austin DW, Takano K, Raes F. Psychopathology and episodic future thinking: a systematic review and meta-analysis of specificity and episodic detail. Behav Res Ther. 2018;102:42–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.01.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Mehu M, Scherer KR. The appraisal bias model of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Emot Rev. 2015;7(3):272–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915575406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Moustafa AA, Morris AN, ElHaj M. A review on future episodic thinking in mood and anxiety disorders. Rev Neurosci. 2019;30(1):85–94. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2017-0055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. van Randenborgh A, de Jong-Meyer R, Hüffmeier J. Rumination fosters indecision in dysphoria. J Clin Psychol. 2010;66(3):229–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/JCLP.20647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Mathews A, Mackintosh B, Fulcher EP. Cognitive biases in anxiety and attention to threat. Trends Cogn Sci. 1997;1(9):340–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01092-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Amir N, Beard C, Bower E. Interpretation bias and social anxiety. Cogn Ther Res. 2005;29(4):433–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10608-005-2834-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(1):1–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cisler JM, Koster EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):203–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2009.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Eysenck M, Mathews A, Mogg K. Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cogn Emot. 2008;6(2):149–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Puliafico AC, Kendall PC. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious youth: a review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2006;9(3):162–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10567-006-0009-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Wu JQ, Szpunar KK, Godovich SA, Schacter DL, Hofmann SG. Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;36:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.09.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Du JY, Hallford DJ, Busby Grant J. Characteristics of episodic future thinking in anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2022;95:102162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Grupe DW. Decision-making in anxiety and its disorders. In: Dreher J-C, Tremblay L, editors. Decision neuroscience: an integrative perspective. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2017. p. 327–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-805308-9.00026-9.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  37. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Cortese S, Fairchild G, Stringaris A. Annual research review: transdiagnostic neuroscience of child and adolescent mental disorders—differentiating decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57(3):321–49. https://doi.org/10.1111/JCPP.12496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Courtney EA, Kushwaha M, Johnson JG. Childhood emotional abuse and risk for hopelessness and depressive symptoms during adolescence. J Emot Abus. 2008;8(3):281–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926790802262572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Dorn LD, Susman EJ, Petersen AC. Cortisol reactivity and anxiety and depression in pregnant adolescents: a longitudinal perspective. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1993;18(3):219.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Gerard JM, Booth MZ. Family and school influences on adolescents’ adjustment: the moderating role of youth hopefulness and aspirations for the future. J Adolesc. 2015;44:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Granö N, Karjalainen M, Ranta K, Lindgren M, Roine M, Therman S. Community-oriented family-based intervention superior to standard treatment in improving depression, hopelessness and functioning among adolescents with any psychosis-risk symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2016;237:9–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Siler M. A prospective test of the hopelessness theory of depression in adolescence. Cogn Ther Res. 2001;25(5):607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Horwitz AG, Berona J, Czyz EK, Yeguez CE, King CA. Positive and negative expectations of hopelessness as longitudinal predictors of depression, suicidal ideation, and suicidal behavior in high-risk adolescents. Suicide Life-Threat Behav. 2017;47(2):168–76. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Kennard BD, Stewart SM, Hughes JL, Patel PG, Emslie GJ. Cognitions and depressive symptoms among ethnic minority adolescents. Cult Divers Ethn Minor Psychol. 2006;12(3):578–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.12.3.578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Li Z, Wang YI, Mao X, Yin X. Relationship between hope and depression in college students: a cross-lagged regression analysis. Personal Ment Health. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Oettingen G, Mayer D, Portnow S. Pleasure now, pain later: positive fantasies about the future predict symptoms of depression. Psychol Sci. 2016;27(3):345–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615620783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Sasser J, Duprey EB, Oshri A. A longitudinal investigation of protective factors for bereaved maltreated youth. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Troister T, Davis MP, Lowndes A, Holden RR. A five-month longitudinal study of psychache and suicide ideation: replication in general and high-risk university students. Suicide Life-Threat Behav. 2013;43(6):611–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. MacLeod AK, Salaminiou E. Reduced positive future-thinking in depression: cognitive and affective factors. Cogn Emot. 2001;15(1):99–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930125776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. MacLeod AK, Tata P, Kentish J, Jacobsen H. Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cogn Emot. 2010;11(4):467–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999397379881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Miles H, MacLeod AK, Pote H. Retrospective and prospective cognitions in adolescents: anxiety, depression, and positive and negative affect. J Adolesc. 2004;27(6):691–701. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADOLESCENCE.2004.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Cummings CM, Caporino NE, Kendall PC. Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents: 20 years after. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(3):816–45. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0034733.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Rohde P, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Comorbidity of unipolar depression: II. Comorbidity with other mental disorders in adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100(2):214–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980–8. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3(2):77–101. https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706QP063OA.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Braun V, Clarke V. Conceptual and design thinking for thematic analysis. Qual Psychol. 2022;9(1):3–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/qup0000196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Frost NA, Nolas S-M. Exploring and Expanding on pluralism in qualitative research in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2011;8(2):115–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/14780887.2011.572728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bhaskar R, Hartwig M. The formation of critical realism: a personal perspective. London: Routledge; 2010.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  59. Willis MEH. Critical realism and qualitative research in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2023;20(2):265–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/14780887.2022.2157782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Tong A, Sainsbury P, Craig J. Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): a 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int J Qual Health Care. 2007;19(6):349–57. https://doi.org/10.1093/INTQHC/MZM042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Yardley L. Dilemmas in qualitative health research. Psychol Health. 2000;15(2):215–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440008400302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Hennink M, Kaiser BN. Sample sizes for saturation in qualitative research: a systematic review of empirical tests. Soc Sci Med. 2022;292:114523. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SOCSCIMED.2021.114523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. McCabe C. Linking anhedonia symptoms with behavioural and neural reward responses in adolescent depression. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;22:143–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2018.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Rzepa E, Fisk J, McCabe C. Blunted neural response to anticipation, effort and consummation of reward and aversion in adolescents with depression symptomatology. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31(3):303–11. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116681416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Watson R, Harvey K, McCabe C, Reynolds S. Understanding anhedonia: a qualitative study exploring loss of interest and pleasure in adolescent depression. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;29(4):489–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00787-019-01364-Y/FIGURES/2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Behar E, McGowan SK, McLaughlin KA, Borkovec TD, Goldwin M, Bjorkquist O. Concreteness of positive, negative, and neutral repetitive thinking about the future. Behav Ther. 2012;43(2):300–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Coughlin C, Lyons KE, Ghetti S. Remembering the past to envision the future in middle childhood: developmental linkages between prospection and episodic memory. Cogn Dev. 2014;30(1):96–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. D’Argembeau A, van der Linden M. Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: the effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Conscious Cogn. 2006;15(2):342–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Marks DF. Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br J Psychol. 1973;64(1):17–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Söderlund H, Moscovitch M, Kumar N, Daskalakis ZJ, Flint A, Herrmann N, Levine B. Autobiographical episodic memory in major depressive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(1):51–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Eysenck MW, Payne S, Santos R. Anxiety and depression: past, present, and future events. Cogn Emot. 2006;20(2):274–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500220066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. MacLeod AK, Tata P, Kentish J, Jacobsen H. Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cogn Emot. 1997;11(4):467–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999397379881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Zheng L, Lippke S, Chen Y, Li D, Gan Y. Future orientation buffers depression in daily and specific stress. PsyCh J. 2019;8(3):342–52. https://doi.org/10.1002/PCHJ.283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Williams K, Snytte J, Sheldon S. Individual differences in depression are reflected in negative self-evaluations when imagining future events. J Behav Cogn Ther. 2022;32(3):207–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbct.2022.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Bjärehed J, Sarkohi A, Andersson G. Less positive or more negative? Future-directed thinking in mild to moderate depression. Cogn Behav Ther. 2010;39(1):37–45. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506070902966926.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Gallagher S. Time, emotion, and depression. Emot Rev. 2012;4(2):127–32. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911430142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Loas G, Salinas E, Pierson A, Guelfi JD, Samuel-Lajeunesse B. Anhedonia and blunted affect in major depressive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 1994;35(5):366–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-440X(94)90277-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Rinaldi L, Locati F, Parolin L, Girelli L. Distancing the present self from the past and the future: psychological distance in anxiety and depression. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017;70(7):1106–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Sica LS, Crocetti E, Ragozini G, Aleni Sestito L, Serafini T. Future-oriented or present-focused? The role of social support and identity styles on ‘futuring’ in Italian late adolescents and emerging adults. J Youth Stud. 2016;19(2):183–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/13676261.2015.1059925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Fakkel M, Peeters M, Branje S, Stevens GWJM, Vollebergh WAM. Decline in positive future orientations among adolescents during covid-19: The role of socioeconomic status, parental support, and sense of control. J Adolesc. 2023;95(7):1321–32. https://doi.org/10.1002/JAD.12204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Loh JMI, Schutte NS, Thorsteinsson EB. Be happy: the role of resilience between characteristic affect and symptoms of depression. J Happiness Stud. 2014;15(5):1125–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10902-013-9467-2/TABLES/5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Waugh CE, Koster EHW. A resilience framework for promoting stable remission from depression. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;41:49–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2014.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Tang P, Sonuga-Barke E, Kostyrka-Allchorne K, Phillips-Owen J. Young people’s future thinking and mental health: the development and validation of the a dolescent future thinking rating scale. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1994.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Bogaert L, Hallford D, Loyen E, D’Argembeau A, Raes F. The potential of future event specificity training (FEST) to decrease anhedonia and dampening of positive emotions in undergraduate students. 2023.

  85. Hallford DJ, Sharma MK, Austin DW. Increasing anticipatory pleasure in major depression through enhancing episodic future thinking: a randomized single-case series trial. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2020;42(4):751–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-020-09820-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Mufson L, Weissman MM, Moreau D, Garfinkel R. Efficacy of interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.6.573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Mychailyszyn MP, Elson DM. Working through the blues: a meta-analysis on interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed adolescents (IPT-A). Child Youth Serv Rev. 2018;87:123–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.02.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Vilhauer JS, Young S, Kealoha C, Borrmann J, IsHak WW, Rapaport MH, Hartoonian N, Mirocha J. Treating major depression by creating positive expectations for the future: a pilot study for the effectiveness of future-directed therapy (FDT) on symptom severity and quality of life. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2012;18(2):102–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-5949.2011.00235.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants who took part in the study for their support and contribution.

Funding

PT is supported by a joint PhD scholarship between King’s College London and China Studentship Council. The funding source had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, interpretation or writing of the report. This is independent research carried out at the National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR) Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre (BRC).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PT: conceptualization; data curation; formal analysis; investigation; methodology; project administration; visualization; writing—original draft; writing—review and editing. GP: methodology; supervision; writing—review and editing. KKA: conceptualization; methodology; supervision; writing—review and editing. JPO: conceptualization; methodology; supervision; writing—review and editing. ESB: conceptualization; methodology; supervision; writing—review and editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peiyao Tang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The Studies in the article have obtained full ethical approval from the Health Faculties Research Ethics Sub-committee at King’s College London, reference number HR/DP-20/21-21394. All participants provided written consent prior to participation in the study.

Consent for publication

All identifiable information has been removed for individual participants. All participants have given written consent for their data to be used in a publication.

Competing interests

The authors report no financial, non-financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tang, P., Pavlopoulou, G., Kostyrka-Allchorne, K. et al. Links between mental health problems and future thinking from the perspective of adolescents with experience of depression and anxiety: a qualitative study. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 17, 143 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00679-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00679-8

Keywords