Skip to main content

Callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors in left-behind preschool children: the role of emotional lability/negativity and positive teacher-child relationship

Abstract

Background

Callous-unemotional traits and emotional lability/negativity of young children have been regarded as the markers of externalizing problem behaviors. Based on the sensitivity to threat and affiliative reward model and the general aggression model, emotional lability/negativity may act as a mediator in the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors. Additionally, a positive teacher-child relationship could act as a buffer given the parental absence in left-behind children. However, these links remain unexplored in left-behind preschool children. Therefore, this study explored the link between callous-unemotional traits of left-behind preschool children and externalizing problem behaviors, as well as the mediating role of emotional lability/negativity and the moderating role of a positive teacher-child relationship.

Method

Data were collected on 525 left-behind children aged 3 to 6 years from rural kindergartens in China. Preschool teachers reported all data through an online survey platform. Moderated mediation analysis was performed to examine whether the mediated relation between callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors was moderated by a positive teacher-child relationship.

Results

The results showed callous-unemotional traits significantly predicted externalizing problem behaviors and lability/negativity acted as a mediator, while a positive teacher-child relationship acted as a protective factor in moderating the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and emotional lability/negativity. This study identified a moderated mediation effect among the four variables in left-behind preschool children in China.

Conclusion

The findings provide support for the advancement of theoretical foundations, and provide an avenue for further exploration to support the mental health and overall development of left-behind children during early childhood.

Introduction

In China, left-behind children (LBC) emerged with the economic and social development of the country. Since the reform and opening up, the uneven development of the region has led to large-scale migration and mobility of labor. LBC emerged when migrant parents left their children in their place of origin to be cared for by grandparents or other guardians. Preschool LBC mainly refer to children whose parents have been working abroad for a long time before the age of 6 and have not yet received compulsory education [1], which is about 20% or more of the total number of LBC in China [2]. The neglect of LBC by migrated parents has had an emotional impact on these children [3]. The insecure attachments caused by less parental supervision and overbearing or overly indulgent care from other guardians may prompt the excessive introversion or egotism of LBC emotionally [4], and more pronounced behavioral problems [5]. Early childhood is a critical stage in a child’s emotional and social development, providing an important foundation for future school adjustment and positive interpersonal relationships [6].

The general aggression model (GAM) proposes that conduct disorders is the outcome of environmental factors and self-developmental factors acting on the proximal psychological status of children [7]. In consideration of the findings in previous empirical studies, negative personality traits (e.g., neuroticism and psychoticism) of LBC were more prominent than in non-left-behind children in middle childhood [8]. Studies have found that impulsive personality traits are associated with emotional incompetence [9], and LBC with absent parents showed emotional inability and anxiety [10]. Similarly, callous-unemotional (CU) traits have been proposed as a predictor of conduct disorder, which is considered externalizing behavior as opposed to internalized negative emotional states such as low emotional responsiveness [11]. Based on the GAM, the role of environmental factors cannot be ignored, especially the protective and buffering effects. For example, while the parents of LBC have migrated, a child’s teacher can serve as an attachment figure and play a protective role buffering the effects of negative personality traits, which may be more prominent in LBC, on emotional and behavioural adaptation [12, 13]. However, it remains unclear as to the mechanisms that may link certain individual characteristics or traits to conduct disorders in LBC in early childhood.

Callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors

Externalizing problem behaviors (EPBs) refer to an individual’s explicit and negative out-of-control behaviors, such as aggression, destructiveness, resistance, hyperactivity, and impulsivity [14]. CU traits have been regarded as contributing to EPBs of preschool children [15, 16]. CU traits refer to the personality tendency of low sensitivity to reward and punishment, as well as low empathy or high indifference to others [17]. Based on ecosystem theory, EPBs in LBC have always been a concern [18, 19], especially the effect of individual characteristics on EPBs [20]. Eysenck’s biological theory emphasized the importance of personality in early childhood [15, 21], and the relationship between certain personality traits and externalizing symptoms has been identified among LBC [22]. For example, Children with high CU traits mainly showed a low level of sensitivity to rewards and punishment, and were more likely to exhibit impulsive or destructive behaviors as well [23]. Additionally, previous studies indicated that CU traits uniquely predicted externalizing symptoms (e.g. conduct problems and oppositional behavior) in preschool children [24,25,26]. The previous study have examined the close relationships among insecure parent-child attachment, CU traits and conduct problems [27].

Therefore, young preschool LBC should deserve more attention. It can be surmised that the higher the CU traits, the more likely LBC will exhibit EPBs. Therefore, this study proposed the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 1

CU traits in left-behind children during early childhood would significantly positively predict EPBs.

Emotional lability/negativity as a mediator

As one of the important indicators of socioemotional development, emotional lability/negativity (LN) refers to children’s reacting rapidly to cues that trigger emotions and having difficulty recovering from adverse emotional reactions [28, 29]. Higher levels of LN in children are associated with lower levels of social adaptation [30]. Children’s emotional LN would be impacted by many internal and external factors, including personality traits [31]. Emotional LN co-occurs with CU traits in people with externalizing problems generally [32]. The triarchic model of psychopathy proposes that disinhibition is related to impulsivity and negative affectivity [33]. Studies demonstrated that children’s CU traits correlated with instability in emotional functioning [34, 35]. Individuals with maladaptive personality traits may have difficulty controlling high levels of negative emotions, which could further lead to EPBs [36]. Accordingly, children with high CU traits may have more intense emotional responsiveness that is self-oriented, instead of emotional responses that are other-oriented [11]. The relationships between CU traits and emotional intensity and resilience might be more evident if the child has experienced maltreatment and psychological distress [37]. Furthermore, the literature showed the fluctuating status in emotions of LBC who were left for a long period might be linked to psychoticism [38]. This finding suggests a possible association between CU traits and emotional LN in LBC during early childhood.

GAM suggests that emotional status could play a key role in the nexus of personality traits and externalizing behaviors [7]. The sensitivity to threat and affiliative reward (STAR) model proposes that children in an at-risk context (e.g., maltreatment) would show “reactionary callousness” and experience negative emotionality [39]. Children with early EPBs and high CU traits often have the pattern of negative emotional lability and shifts [40, 41]. Furthermore, emotional LN is regarded as a marker of EPBs in preschoolers [42]. Emotional LN may be the upfront manifestation of EPBs effected by high CU traits, such as the symptoms of LBC [20]. Therefore, this study proposed the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 2

Emotional LN plays a mediating role in the relationship between CU traits and EPBs in left-behind children during early childhood.

Positive teacher-child relationship as a moderator

Based on ecosystem theory, children’s development is related to multiple contextual factors [18, 43], such as the teacher-child relationship (TCR) [44, 45]. TCR refers to the psychological multisystem formed between young children and teachers in kindergarten, with emotional, cognitive, and behavioral interactions as the main manifestations [46]. As a contextual factor, TCR could positively predict preschool children’s social and emotional adjustment [47]. The goodness-of-fit model proposes that children’s temperaments interact with their external contextual factors to affect children’s development [48]. As such, a positive TCR would partially compensate for the negative effects of CU traits [49]. For instance, a positive association between CU traits and punishment insensitivity to teachers has been found [50], as well as a negative relationship between a positive TCR and CU traits [51, 52]. Meanwhile, an empirical study found the interactive effect of these two variables on emotional and behavioral adjustment in preschool children [53]. Based on the GAM, an interactive effect of contextual factors and personality traits on internal emotional states can be proposed [7]. One study found that the TCR had a moderating impact on the relationship between temperamental characteristics and emotional functioning [54]. Additionally, there was a significant pairwise connection among TCR, CU traits, and emotional LN in children [35].

From the contribution of attachment theory to positive TCR, the teacher was perceived as an unique attachment figure who could provide a safe haven and the function of seeking comfort for young children [55]. TCR quality played a buffering role in the positive association between poor parental monitoring and low emotional control in children [56]. LBC may lack opportunities for parent-child interaction, while teachers, as an important attachment figure, may have a positive effect on the development of children’s prosocial emotions by forming a positive TCR [12, 57]. Though the number of studies limited, existing studies implied that positive TCR might reduce CU trait development increasingly [58,59,60], especially for children with insecure attachment experiences [61]. Therefore, this study proposed the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 3

A positive TCR plays a moderating role in the link between CU traits and emotional LN in left-behind children during early childhood.

The current study

In order to explore the underlying mechanisms in the association between CU traits and EPBs in left-behind children during early childhood, this study explored the mediating role of LN and the moderating role of TCR. Specifically, the following three hypotheses were tested: (1) CU traits would significantly positively predict EPBs, (2) emotional LN plays a mediation effect in the relationship between CU traits and EPBs, and (3) a positive TCR plays a moderating role in the relationship between CU traits and emotional LN. The results of the current study could provide empirical support for the GAM, a better understanding of factors associated with the social adaptation of LBC in early childhood, and a comprehensive perspective to promote their emotional and behavioral development.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Purposeful or convenience sampling was used to recruit kindergarten teachers of LBC aged 3–6 in rural areas of Guangdong province, China. A total of 638 questionnaires were distributed through Wenjuanxing as the Chinese online survey platform (http://www.wjx.cn, accessed on Jan 15, 2023). As the attachment figure of LBC, kindergarten teachers report all data. After excluding invalid questionnaires (e.g., missing some items, short duration and answer inconsistency obviously), 525 were included in the analysis (response rate of 82.3%). Among them, 265 were boys (50.50%, Mage=4.20, SD = 0.81) and 260 girls (49.50%, Mage=4.22, SD = 0.87). The study was reviewed approved by the research ethics committee of Guangzhou University (Protocol Number: GZHU202301).

Measures

Callous-unemotional traits

The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU) [62] was used to evaluate CU traits. The Chinese short version has been revised and includes two dimensions: uncaring and callousness [63]. The questionnaire has 11 items (e.g., He/she seems cold and inconsiderate) that are responded to using a 4-point scale (scored 1–4). The higher the score, the higher the degree of CU traits. Previous studies have shown the scale has great reliability and validity in Chinese preschool children [64, 65]. In this study, the Cronbach’s α of the ICU was 0.78, KMO = 0.84, and the Bartlett test p < 0.001.

Emotional lability/negativity

Emotional LN was measured using the Emotional Lability/Negativity Scale [66]. In this study, the revised Chinese version of this scale was used to assess emotional LN, which consists of 7 items that are responded to using a 4-point scale [67]. The items primarily assess emotional flexibility, dysregulation, and unpredictability of negative emotions. The scale has demonstrated good reliability and validity in previous research with Chinese preschool children [68]. In this study, Cronbach’s α was 0.88, KMO = 0.90, and Bartlett test p < 0.001.

Externalizing problem behaviors

According to existing studies [69,70,71], EPBs was measured based on the two scales of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (i.e. conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention). Each scale has five items (conduct problems: e.g., often fights, lies or cheats, and hyperactivity/inattention: e.g., restless, overactive, unable to stay still for long) that are responded to using a 3-point scale (scored 0–2 points). Higher scale scores indicate more externalizing behaviors. Previous studies indicate the SDQ has good reliability and validity [72]. In this study, Cronbach’s α of the subscale was 0.73, KMO = 0.83, and the Bartlett test p < 0.001.

Teacher-child relationship

The TCR was evaluated using the Chinese version of the Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS) [73, 74], which consists of 28 items that are responded to using a 5-point scale (scored 1–5 points). Due to the low reliability of the Dependency subscale in the Chinese social context, only the subscales assessing teacher-child closeness and teacher-child conflict were used [75]. The Conflict (8 items) and Closeness (7 items) subscales consist of 15 items total. Higher scores indicate a more positive teacher-child relationship. Previous studies have demonstrated that the 15-item STRS has good reliability and validity [76]. In this study, Cronbach’s α = 0.86, KMO = 0.91, and the Bartlett test p < 0.001.

Data processing and analysis

All statistical analysis of data was conducted by SPSS 26.0. and its macro program. First, a Pearson correlation matrix that included young LBC’s CU traits, emotional LN, EPBs, and positive TCR was constructed. Second, two macro-Model were used in the further analysis [77]. According to the hypothesis 1 and 2, PROCESS Model 4 was performed to examine the mediating effect of emotional LN on the link between CU traits and EPBs. The moderating role of TCR in the link of CU traits and emotional LN was tested via PROCESS Model 7. Third, in the parameter test, the Bootstrap method was used to test the significance of the regression coefficient, a total of 5000 samples were constructed, each sample size was 525. The standard deviation and confidence interval of the parameter estimation were obtained. If the 95% confidence interval does not include 0, the result is significant, and vice versa [78].

Results

Preliminary analyses

The results of the difference test showed that there were gender differences in CU traits (t = 2.34, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.20), EPBs (t = 2.49, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.22), and the TCR (t = -2.16, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.19). There was also a significant age difference in CU traits: older age was associated with less CU traits (F = 3.43, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.01). For the precision of the analysis, gender and age were regarded as control variables in subsequent examinations to exclude their effects.

The results of the Pearson correlation analysis are shown in Table 1. CU traits significantly positively correlated with emotional LN (r = 0.54, p < 0.01) and EPBs (r = 0.64, p < 0.01), and emotional LN positively correlated with EPBs (r = 0.61, p < 0.01). In contrast, a positive TCR was negatively associated with CU traits (r = -0.69, p < 0.01), emotional LN (r = -0.46, p < 0.01), and EPBs (r = -0.56, p < 0.01). Thus, higher CU traits in preschool LBC was associated with higher levels of emotional LN and EPBs, whereas a positive TCR was linked with reduced levels of emotional LN and EPBs.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables

Testing for a mediation effect

In order to reveal the mediating role of emotional LN in the relationship between CU traits and EPBs, multiple regression was used controlling for the effects of gender and age. The results showed CU traits significantly positively predicted externalizing problem behaviors (β = 0.54, p < 0.01). Next, the Model 4 macro program was selected to construct a mediation model with emotional LN as a mediator. The direct effect of CU traits on EPBs was significant (Direct effect = 0.44, SE = 0.04, Boot CI = [0.37, 0.52]) and the indirect effect of emotional LN was significant (Indirect effect = 0.20, SE = 0.03, Boot CI = [0.15, 0.25]). The effect size of the mediating effect, i.e. the contribution rate of mediation effect in emotional LN expressed as the ratio of indirect effect to total effect, was about 31%. Therefore, emotional LN played a mediating role in the link between CU traits and EPBs.

Testing for moderated mediation

To further investigate whether the TCR plays a moderating role between CU traits as the predictor and emotional LN as the mediator, Macro-Model 7 was selected to test the moderated mediation effect. After controlling for the effects of gender and age, the results (as shown in Table 2) showed that CU traits positively predicted EPBs (β = 0.44, p < 0.01), emotional LN positively predicted EPBs (β = 0.36, p < 0.01), and CU traits had a significant positive predictive effect on emotional LN (β = 0.43, p < 0.01). However, positive TCR negatively predicted emotional LN (β = -0.17, p < 0.01). Moreover, the interaction effect of CU traits and TCR negatively predicted emotional LN (β = -0.08, p < 0.05). Thus, the TCR moderated the link of CU traits and emotional LN.

Table 2 Evaluating the moderated mediation effects: emotional LN as the moderator

In order to explore the moderating effect of the TCR, the score for TCR was divided into three conditions: high, medium, and low. As shown in Fig. 1, the effect values and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals for CU traits, emotional LN, and a positive TCR validated a moderated mediation model. The more positive the quality of relationship between young LBC and their teachers, the more subtle the negative effect of CU traits on emotional LN. In summary, it can be concluded that emotional LN serves as a mediator between CU traits and EPBs in preschool LBC, and the first half of the pathway is moderated by a positive TCR, which reduces the impact of CU traits on emotional LN.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Moderating effect of positive TCR

Discussion

This study explored the mechanism behind a correlation of CU traits and EPBs in preschool LBC in China. Based on existing theoretical models and empirical studies, the indirect effects of emotional LN and TCR were proposed and verified. The results showed that in left-behind children during early childhood, CU traits can significantly positively affect the occurrence of EPBs, and in this process, emotional LN plays a mediating role significantly; however, a positive TCR can reduce emotional LN in children, in turn reducing the frequency of EPBs.

Social adaptation is a topic that has been the focus of research on LBC, especially externalizing behaviors. Previous studies have explored predictors of externalizing behaviors in LBC, such as social support and personality traits [22, 79, 80]. Research involving externalizing behaviors and CU traits have been conducted mainly with school-age children and adolescents, and even adults. However, it was unclear whether CU traits would have an impact on externalizing behaviors in preschool children, especially in LBC. The results of this study revealed that CU traits profoundly influenced externalizing behaviors (Hypothesis 1), which is consistent with the results of previous studies [81,82,83]. This also suggests that CU traits are an important marker variable for externalizing disorders during a critical stage of personality development and early childhood socialization [53].

EPBs could have a negative effect on LBC’s academic performance and future social adjustment [84, 85]. Personality traits (e,g. CU traits) in LBC have received growing attention [8], and CU traits have been found to be predictive of social adaption and externalizing problems in early childhood [15, 53]. Consequently, the findings of this study offer new evidence of the possible effect of CU traits on LBC’s behavior during early childhood.

The association between CU traits and externalizing behaviors in preschool LBC was demonstrated to be mediated by emotional LN in this study (Hypothesis 2), which is consistent with previous research on the mediating role of emotional LN between personality traits and behavioral disorders in children [36, 86, 87]. The results support the GAM, which proposes that individual factors trigger impulsive behaviors through psychological and emotional arousal [7]. In addition, the findings provide empirical support for the STAR model [39]. Emotional LN in preschool LBC positively predicted externalizing symptoms, which is consistent with the findings of previous studies on general preschool children [42, 88]. Children with high CU traits may show impulsivity and experience negative emotionality [33], which tends to be self-oriented [11]. In other words, children with CU traits tend to ignore the feelings of others in social situations, which appears as callousness. However, when related to the self, emotions may explode due to external pressure and lack of self-control [7, 89, 90]. In addition, according to the STAR model [39], left-behind experience during early childhood contributes to an insecure attachment style with migrant parents and left-behind guardians, which may further reinforce the mediating role of emotional LN between CU traits and externalizing behaviors.

A positive TCR was negatively associated with emotional LN, consistent with previous studies [91]. Moreover, the TCR moderated the relationship between CU traits and emotional LN (Hypothesis 3). Previous studies have found TCR moderated the link between children’s temperaments and their emotional response [54, 92]. Firstly, in accordance with the GAM and existing studies [7, 12, 53], the TCR as a protective factor would buffer the negative effects of CU traits on emotional states. Secondly, based on the perspective of attachment, although LBC are disadvantaged when it comes to establishing a secure attachment to their parents, teachers can become an alternative attachment figure for the construction of positive relationships indeed [55, 60]. Thirdly, a good fit between the TCR and the child’s temperament is associated with reduced negative performance in children [48, 93]. A positive TCR acted as a moderator in the present study providing support for the goodness-of-fit model among preschool LBC.

The support of teachers could facilitate the prosocial tendencies of LBC [94]. A positive TCR would play a compensatory role among LBC in the cultural context of a supportive environment [95]. In the forming of a high-quality TCR, teachers would support emotional expression, especially in young LBC [12]. A positive TCR would involve creating a warm climate in the classroom and offering children opportunities to exercise self-control when experiencing negative emotions [96,97,98]. Furthermore, emotional LN was deemed as the portent of EPBs as well [42]. In this regard, changes in emotionality may be acted out and be seen. It is reasonable to enhance the level of positive TCR to reduce emotional lability in preschool children. For preschool LBC with high CU traits, a more positive TCR should be established, so that the probability of negative emotionality will decrease, and the prevalence of EPBs will be reduced. However, further research on the association of CU traits and positive TCR is needed to understand how to enhance the social and emotional development of left-behind children during early childhood.

Conclusion

This study examined the links between CU traits and externalizing symptoms in preschool left-behind children. The results showed that CU traits could predict externalizing behaviors, and that emotional LN acted as a mediator while the TCR moderated the connection between CU traits and emotional LN. In short, the study identified CU traits as a marker of EPBs, and underscored the importance of emotional lability/negativity and establishment of a high-quality teacher-child relationship as contributing to the dynamic association between CU traits and EPBs. The teacher-child relationship deserves more attention in future interventions for preschool left-behind children.

Data Availability

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CU:

Callous-unemotional

EPBs:

Externalizing problem behaviors

GAM:

General aggression model

LBC:

Left-behind children

LN:

Emotional lability/negativity

STAR:

Sensitivity to threat and affiliative reward

TCR:

Teacher-child relationship

References

  1. Xiao J, Liu X. How does family cultural capital influence the individuals’ development? —case study about left-behind children in China. Asia Pac Educ Rev. 2023;24(1):167–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12564-022-09744-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ministry of Civil Affairs. Chart: Data of Rural Left-Behind Children in 2018. Ministry of Civil Affairs of the people’s republic of China; Published September 2018. https://www.Mca.Gov.Cn/article/gk/tjtb/201809/20180900010882.Shtml. Accessed March 20, 2023.

  3. Wen YJ, Hou WP, Zheng W, Zhao XX, Wang XQ, Bo QJ, Pao C, Tang YL, Tan T, Li XB, Wang CY. The neglect of left-behind children in China: a meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abus. 2021;22(5):1326–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838020916837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Man Y, Mengmeng L, Lezhi L, Ting M, Jingping Z. The psychological problems and related influential factors of left-behind adolescents (LBA) in Hunan, China: a cross sectional study. Int J Equity Health. 2017;16(1):163. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-017-0639-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Fan F, Su L, Gill MK, Birmaher B. Emotional and behavioral problems of chinese left-behind children: a preliminary study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(6):655–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-009-0107-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Denham SA, Bassett HH, Thayer SK, Mincic MS, Sirotkin YS, Zinsser K. Observing preschoolers’ social-emotional behavior: structure, foundations, and prediction of early school success. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173(3):246–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2011.597457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(5):353–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00366.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wang L, Wu W, Qu G, Tang X, Sun Y. The personality traits of left-behind children in China: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Health Med. 2019;24(3):253–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2018.1540787.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gratz KL, Rosenthal MZ, Tull MT, Lejuez CW, Gunderson JG. An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115(4):850–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Liu Z, Li X, Ge X. Left too early: the effects of age at separation from parents on chinese rural children’s symptoms of anxiety and depression. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(11):2049–54. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2008.150474.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Northam JC, Dadds MR. Is callous always cold? A critical review of the literature on emotion and the development of callous-unemotional traits in children. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2020;23(2):265–83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-019-00309-w.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Liu Y, Li X, Chen L, Qu Z. Perceived positive teacher-student relationship as a protective factor for chinese left-behind children’s emotional and behavioural adjustment. Int J Psychol. 2015;50(5):354–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Guo J, Ren X, Wang X, Qu Z, Zhou Q, Ran C, Wang X, Hu J. Depression among migrant and left-behind children in China in relation to the quality of parent-child and teacher-child relationships. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(12):e0145606. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145606.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. The classification of child psychopathology: a review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psychol Bull. 1978;85(6):1275–301. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1275.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Longman T, Hawes DJ, Kohlhoff J. Callous-unemotional traits as markers for conduct problem severity in early childhood: a meta-analysis. Child Psychiat Hum Dev. 2016;47(2):326–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-015-0564-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Zhang J, Li W, Zhang H, Wilson A, Shuai L, Xia W, Wang Z, Qiu M, Wang Y. Callous-unemotional traits in chinese preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2021;15(1):35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-021-00388-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(4):359–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bronfenbrenner U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: research perspectives. Dev Psychol. 1986;22(6):723. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Cox MJ, Paley B. Families as systems. Annu Rev Psychol. 1997;48:243–67. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.243.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Dong Y, Liu F, Jiang Y, Wei S. Neuroticism and aggressive behavior among left-behind children: the mediating roles of interpersonal sensitivity and bullying victimization. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19:11072. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191711072.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Eysenck HJ. The Biological basis of personality. London: Routledge; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Yang F, Jin Z, He J, Han B, Huang X, Chen K, Wang J. Aggressive behaviors and associated factors in chinese left-behind adolescents: a cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 2022;22(1):677. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-022-03736-x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Allen JL, Bird E, Chhoa CY. Bad boys and mean girls: Callous-unemotional traits, management of disruptive behavior in school, the teacher-student relationship and academic motivation. Front Educ. 2018;3:108. https://doi.10.3389/feduc.2018.00108.

  24. Waller R, Hyde LW, Grabell AS, Alves ML, Olson SL. Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(6):657–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Bansal PS, Goh PK, Eng AG, Elkins AR, Thaxton M, Martel MM. Utility of the limited prosocial emotions specifier in preschoolers with conduct problems. Assessment. 2023;30(2):274–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911211051070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Barry CT, Frick PJ, DeShazo TM, McCoy M, Ellis M, Loney BR. The importance of callous–unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(2):335. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843X.109.2.335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Attachment and callous-unemotional traits in children with early-onset conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):838–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02544.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Dunsmore JC, Booker JA, Ollendick TH. Parental emotion coaching and child emotion regulation as protective factors for children with oppositional defiant disorder. Soc Dev. 2013;22(3):444–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00652.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kim-Spoon J, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. A longitudinal study of emotion regulation, emotion lability-negativity, and internalizing symptomatology in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child Dev. 2013;84(2):512–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01857.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Murphy B, Maszk P, Smith M, Karbon M. The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 1995;66(5):1360–84. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Kuvaas NJ. The five-factor model of impulsivity-like traits and emotional lability in aggressive behavior. Aggress Behav. 2013;39(3):222–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21474.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Elowsky J, Bajaj S, Bashford-Largo J, Zhang R, Mathur A, Schwartz A, Dobbertin M, Blair KS, Leibenluft E, Pardini D, Blair RJR. Differential associations of conduct disorder, callous-unemotional traits and irritability with outcome expectations and values regarding the consequences of aggression. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2022;16(1):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-022-00466-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Patrick CJ, Fowles DC, Krueger RF. Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(3):913–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Ciucci E, Baroncelli A, Golmaryami FN, Frick PJ. The emotional correlates to callous-unemotional traits in children. J Child Fam Stud. 2015;24:2374–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-0040-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Roslyne Wilkinson H, Jones Bartoli A. Antisocial behaviour and teacher-student relationship quality: the role of emotion-related abilities and callous-unemotional traits. Br J Educ Psychol. 2021;91(1):482–99. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ireland JL, Lewis M, Ireland CA, Derefaka G, Taylor L, McBoyle J, Smillie L, Chu S, Archer J. Self-reported psychopathy and aggression motivation: a role for emotions? J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol. 2019;31(1):156–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/14789949.2019.1705376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Cecil CAM, McCrory EJ, Barker ED, Guiney J, Viding E. Characterising youth with callous-unemotional traits and concurrent anxiety: evidence for a high-risk clinical group. Eur Child Adolesc Psych. 2018;27(7):885–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1086-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Xu W, Yan N, Chen G, Zhang X, Feng T. Parent-child separation: the relationship between separation and psychological adjustment among chinese rural children. Qual Life Res. 2018;27(4):913–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-017-1776-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Waller R, Wagner N. The sensitivity to threat and affiliative reward (STAR) model and the development of callous-unemotional traits. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:656–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Rosen PJ, Walerius DM, Fogleman ND, Factor PI. The association of emotional lability and emotional and behavioral difficulties among children with and without ADHD. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2015;7(4):281–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-015-0175-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Graziano PA, Landis T, Maharaj A, Ros-Demarize R, Hart KC, Garcia A. Differentiating preschool children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors through emotion regulation and executive functioning. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2022;51(2):170–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1666399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Maire J, Galéra C, Meyer E, Salla J, Michel G. Is emotional lability a marker for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety and aggression symptoms in preschoolers? Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2017;22(2):77–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lerner RM, Rothbaum F, Boulos S, Castellino DR. Developmental systems perspectiveon parenting. In: Marc HB, editor. Handbook of parenting: Biology and Ecology of Parenting. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2002. pp. 315–39.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Zhang X, Nurmi JE. Teacher-child relationships and social competence: a two-year longitudinal study of chinese preschoolers. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2012;33(3):125–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2012.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Pianta RC. Adult-child relationship processes and early schooling. Early Educ Dev. 1997;8(1):11–26. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed0801_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Pianta RC, Hamre B, Stuhlman M. Relationships between teachers and children. In: William MR, Gloria EM, editors. Comprehensive handbook of psychology: Educational psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2003. pp. 199–234.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Baker JA, Grant S, Morlock L. The teacher-student relationship as a developmental context for children with internalizing or externalizing behavior problems. Sch Psychol Q. 2008;23(1):3–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Chess S, Thomas A. Temperament and the Concept of Goodness of Fit. In: Strelau J, Angleitner A, editors. Explorations in temperament: International Perspectives on individual differences. Boston, MA: Springer; 1991. pp. 15–28.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  49. Bégin V, Fontaine NMG, Vitaro F, Boivin M, Tremblay RE, Côté SM. Childhood psychopathic traits and mental health outcomes in adolescence: compensatory and protective effects of positive relationships with parents and teachers. Eur Child Adolesc Psych. 2022;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-01955-2.

  50. Allen JL, Morris A, Chhoa CY. Callous-unemotional (CU) traits in adolescent boys and response to teacher reward and discipline strategies. Emot Behav Diffic. 2016;21(3):329–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632752.2016.1165968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Baroncelli A, Ciucci E. Bidirectional effects between callous-unemotional traits and student-teacher relationship quality among middle school students. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(2):277–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00595-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Hwang S, Waller R, Hawes DJ, Allen JL. Longitudinal associations between callous-unemotional (CU) traits and school-based affiliative relationships among south korean children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2022;51(4):556–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2021.1881904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Zhu J, Xia X, Wu Q, Zou S, Li Y. Callous-unemotional traits and social adjustment among chinese preschoolers: the moderating role of teacher-child relationship. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(4):3426. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043426.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Susa-Erdogan G, Benga O, Albu-Răduleț M, Macovei T. Child temperament and child-teacher relationship quality: implications for children’s emotional functioning during preschool period. Front Psychol. 2022;13:992292. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.992292.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Verschueren K, Koomen HM. Teacher-child relationships from an attachment perspective. Attach Hum Dev. 2012;14(3):205–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2012.672260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Fisher JH, Brown JL. A prospective, longitudinal examination of the influence of childhood home and school contexts on psychopathic characteristics in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2018;47(10):2041–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0861-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Vaish A, Hepach R. The development of prosocial emotions. Emot Rev. 2020;12(4):259–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919885014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Baroncelli A, Facci C, Sica LS, Fusco L, Di Palma T, Ciucci E. Attachment to others and callous-unemotional traits in a sample of high school students. Curr Psychol. 2023;1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04237-5.

  59. Hughes JN, Cavell TA. Influence of the teacher-student relationship in childhood conduct problems: a prospective study. J Clin Child Psychol. 1999;28(2):173–84. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2802_5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Cao X, Somerville MP, Allen JL. Teachers’ perceptions of the school functioning of chinese preschool children with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behaviors. Teach Teach Educ. 2023;123:103990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2022.103990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Stoppelbein L, McRae E, Smith S. Examining student-teacher relationship and callous-unemotional traits in children with adverse childhood experiences. School Ment Health. 2021;13:129–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-020-09397-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Frick PJ. Inventory of callous-unemotional traits. Unpublished rating scale. New Orleans, LA: University of New Orleans; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Wang MC, Gao Y, Deng J, Lai H, Deng Q, Armour C. The factor structure and construct validity of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits in chinese undergraduate students. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(12):e0189003. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Mertan B, Goulter N, Katsimicha E. Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44(4):625–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0075-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Deng QW, Ding CX, Liu ML, Deng JX, Wang MC. Psychometric properties of the inventory of callous-unemotional in preschool students. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2016;24(4):663–6.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Shields A, Cicchetti D. Emotion regulation among school-age children: the development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Dev Psychol. 1997;33(6):906–16. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.906.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Zeng YB. An investigation on the status quo of emotion regulation development in preschool children. Chengdu: Sichuan Normal University; 2020. pp. 27–30.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Zhu JJ, Yan YC, Yang TT, Zhu L, Wu M, Wang YJ, Li Y. Reliability and validity of the emotion regulation checklist to chinese preschoolers. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2020;28(6):1186–9.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Int Rev Psych. 2003;15(1–2):166–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/0954026021000046128.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Vugteveen J, de Bildt A, Timmerman ME. Normative data for the self-reported and parent-reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) for ages 12–17. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2022;16(1):1–13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-021-00437-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Kishida K, Hida N, Ishikawa SI. Evaluating the effectiveness of a transdiagnostic universal prevention program for both internalizing and externalizing problems in children: two feasibility studies. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2022;16(1):9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-022-00445-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Goodman R, Slobodskaya H, Knyazev G. Russian child mental health: a cross-sectional study of prevalence and risk factors. Eur Child Adolesc Psych. 2005;14(1):28–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-005-0420-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Pianta RC, Steinberg M. Teacher-child relationships and the process of adjusting to school. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. 1992;57:61–80. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219925706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Zhang X. Reliability and validity of teacher-child relationship scale. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2010;15:582–3.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Aboagye MO, Qin J, Pekárková S, Antwi CO, Jababu Y, Asare K, Affum-Osei E, Akinyi N. Factorial validity of the student-teacher relationship scale–short form, latent means comparison of teacher-student relationship quality and association with child problem and prosocial behaviours. Psychol Stud. 2019;64(2):221–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-019-00488-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Liu T, Zhang X, Zhao K, Chan WL. Teacher-child relationship quality and chinese toddlers’ developmental functioning: a cross-lagged modelling approach. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;116:105192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Hayes AF. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. 2012. http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.

  78. Erceg-Hurn DM, Mirosevich VM. Modern robust statistical methods: an easy way to maximize the accuracy and power of your research. Am Psychol. 2008;63(7):591–601. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.591.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Zhang R, Qiu Z, Li Y, Liu L, Zhi S. Teacher support, peer support, and externalizing problems among left-behind children in rural china: sequential mediation by self-esteem and self-control. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2021;121:105824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Otto C, Kaman A, Erhart M, Barkmann C, Klasen F, Schlack R, Ravens-Sieberer U. Risk and resource factors of antisocial behaviour in children and adolescents: results of the longitudinal BELLA study. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2021;15(1):61. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-021-00412-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Castagna PJ, Babinski DE, Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA. The significance of limited prosocial emotions among externalizing disorders in children. Eur Child Adolesc Psych. 2022;31(4):589–600. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01696-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Waller R, Dishion TJ, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Wilson MN, Hyde LW. Does early childhood callous-unemotional behavior uniquely predict behavior problems or callous-unemotional behavior in late childhood? Dev Psychol. 2016;52(11):1805–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000165.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Zumbach J, Rademacher A, Koglin U. Conceptualizing callous-unemotional traits in preschoolers: Associations with social-emotional competencies and aggressive behavior. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2021;15(1):24. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-021-00376-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Moilanen KL, Shaw DS, Maxwell KL. Developmental cascades: externalizing, internalizing, and academic competence from middle childhood to early adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22(3):635–53. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000337.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Reef J, Diamantopoulou S, van Meurs I, Verhulst FC, van der Ende J. Developmental trajectories of child to adolescent externalizing behavior and adult DSM-IV disorder: results of a 24-year longitudinal study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(12):1233–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-010-0297-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Edwards ER, Rose NLJ, Gromatsky M, Feinberg A, Kimhy D, Doucette JT, Goodman M, McClure MM, Perez-Rodriguez MM, New AS, Hazlett EA. Alexithymia, affective lability, impulsivity, and childhood adversity in borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2021;35:114–31. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2021_35_513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Liu J, Ran G, Zhang Q, Li Y, Zhang Q. The association between callous-unemotional traits and suicide ideation among youth: a conditional process analysis. J Affect Disord. 2023;328:245–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.02.063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Overgaard KR, Oerbeck B, Aase H, Torgersen S, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Zeiner P. Emotional lability in preschoolers with symptoms of ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2018;22(8):787–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715576342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Fang J, Wang X, Yuan KH, Wen Z. Childhood psychological maltreatment and moral disengagement: a moderated mediation model of callous-unemotional traits and empathy. Pers Individ Differ. 2020;157:109814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.109814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Hadjicharalambous MZ, Fanti KA. Self regulation, cognitive capacity and risk taking: investigating heterogeneity among adolescents with callous-unemotional traits. Child Psychiat Hum Dev. 2018;49(3):331–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-017-0753-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Pallini S, Vecchio GM, Baiocco R, Schneider BH, Laghi F. Student-teacher relationships and attention problems in school-aged children: the mediating role of emotion regulation. School Ment Health. 2018;11(2):309–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-018-9286-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Acar IH, Torquati JC, Raikes H, Rudasill KM. Pathways to low-income children’s self-regulation: child temperament and the qualities of teacher-child relationships. Early Educ Dev. 2020;32(8):1103–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2020.1830465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Hipson WE, Séguin DG. Is good fit related to good behaviour? Goodness of fit between daycare teacher-child relationships, temperament, and prosocial behaviour. Early Child Dev Care. 2016;186(5):785–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2015.1061518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Lan X. Left-behind youth are not always bad! Relations between teacher autonomy support, narcissism, and prosocial behavior. Curr Psychol. 2022;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03610-0.

  95. Lan X, Moscardino U. Direct and interactive effects of perceived teacher-student relationship and grit on student wellbeing among stay-behind early adolescents in urban China. Learn Individ Differ. 2019;69:129–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Williford AP, Vick Whittaker JE, Vitiello VE, Downer JT. Children’s Engagement within the preschool classroom and their development of self-regulation. Early Educ Dev. 2013;24(2):162–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.628270.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Cadima J, Verschueren K, Leal T, Guedes C. Classroom interactions, dyadic teacher-child relationships, and self-regulation in socially disadvantaged young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44(1):7–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0060-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Nguyen T, Ansari A, Pianta RC, Whittaker JV, Vitiello VE, Ruzek E. The classroom relational environment and children’s early development in preschool. Soc Dev. 2020;29(4):1071–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was funded by Guangdong Office of Philosophy and Social Science (Grant No. GD22CXL02).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the study’s conception and design. SC, RT, XG, GH and XW conducted material preparation and data collection. RT and XG performed and interpreted the data. RT and XG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SC mainly edited and reviewed the whole manuscript, and supervised planning and conducting the study. SC was responsible for the whole project. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suiqing Chen.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was reviewed approved by the research ethics committee of Education School, Guangzhou University (Protocol Number: GZHU202301).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tan, R., Guo, X., Chen, S. et al. Callous-unemotional traits and externalizing problem behaviors in left-behind preschool children: the role of emotional lability/negativity and positive teacher-child relationship. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 17, 82 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00633-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00633-8

Keywords